Online videos indicate human and dog behaviour preceding dog bites and the context in which bites occur

Online videos indicate human and dog behaviour preceding dog bites and the context in which bites occur


Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT YouTube videos of dog bites present an unexplored opportunity to observe dog bites directly. We recorded the context of bites, bite severity, victim and dog characteristics for 143


videos and for 56 videos we coded human and dog behaviour before the bite. Perceived bite severity was derived from visual aspects of the bite. Associations between bite severity and victim,


dog and context characteristics were analysed using a Bayesian hierarchical regression model. Human and dog behaviour before the bite were summarised with descriptive statistics. No


significant differences in bite severity were observed between contexts. Only age of the victim was predictive of bite severity: adults were bitten more severely than infants and infants


more severely than children. Non-neutral codes describing dog body posture and some displacement and appeasement behaviours increased approximately 20 seconds before the bite and humans made


more tactile contacts with dogs 21 seconds before the bite. This analysis can help to improve understanding of context in which bites occur and improve bite prevention by highlighting


observable human and dog behaviours occurring before the bite. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS DOMESTIC DOGS (_CANIS FAMILIARIS_) GRIEVE OVER THE LOSS OF A CONSPECIFIC Article Open


access 24 February 2022 FEAR EXPRESSIONS OF DOGS DURING NEW YEAR FIREWORKS: A VIDEO ANALYSIS Article Open access 29 September 2020 DOG OWNER MENTAL HEALTH IS ASSOCIATED WITH DOG BEHAVIOURAL


PROBLEMS, DOG CARE AND DOG-FACILITATED SOCIAL INTERACTION: A PROSPECTIVE COHORT STUDY Article Open access 08 December 2023 INTRODUCTION Dog bites are a global public health problem resulting


in substantial costs to health care systems1,2,3, and businesses as a result of time off work, human physical and mental health impacts4,5,6,7 and also affect dog welfare, since dogs that


bite are likely to be relinquished to shelters8 and/or euthanised9. Human population-level risk factors associated with dog bites include young age of the victim1,10,11,12,13,14 (but


see15,16) and male sex11 (but see12,15,16). The breed, neuter status and sex of dogs have also been highlighted17, although the link between these factors and bite risk are contested11,15.


Additionally, the physical environment where the interactions are taking place and the dog’s history are suggested risk factors for the occurrence of a bite5,18. Most bites to adults are to


limbs and children receive more bites to the face and neck areas1, regardless of dog size19, suggesting that children interact with dogs differently than adults. As well the risk factors for


the occurrence of a bite, studies have scrutinised the risk factors for severity of a bite. The severity of a bite tends to be greater among older victims, when the victim is not the owner


of the biting dog, when the bite takes place in a public area and outside of the play context15. A link between severity and breed has also been suggested20,21 (but see22), however lack of


clear guidelines for breed identification and small sample sizes makes this finding unreliable and inconclusive23. Improving understanding of what changes the severity of bites is important,


as whilst some bites may be difficult to prevent, reducing their severity may be more achievable. Understanding of the contexts in which dog bites occur is crucial for bite prevention.


Interactions that are often discussed as preceding bites at a population level include those that are likely to be painful or uncomfortable to dogs, such as medical procedures, physical


abuse to dogs16,18, teasing10, interacting with dogs over resources (e.g. food or toys) or on a dog’s perceived territory24, playing with or nearby a dog25 and mundane, daily occurrences


such as petting or reaching towards a dog13,24,26,27. However, a qualitative study illustrated that some bite victims could not explain why they were bitten or were not aware of the dog’s


presence before the bite26, which suggests that identification of interactions before the bite may not be very accurate. Dog bites cannot be studied experimentally as exposing a volunteer to


a bite or provoking a dog to bite would be unethical. As bite incidents are relatively rare, collecting data through real-time observations is not feasible. Therefore, dog bite data is


gathered through general population surveys e.g.28, veterinary caseloads e.g.17, hospital admissions e.g.1 and interviews with dog bite victims e.g.26. The hospital admission datasets are


often large, but the data does not systematically include information about the circumstances of the bite29. Some of the data, e.g. regarding dog’s breed, may also be unreliable or not


recorded23,30. Moreover, in UK hospitals, dog bites are coded under the code “Bitten or struck by dog”, which means that other dog-related incidents, such as falling over because of a dog,


may be included within these statistics1. As only a fraction of bites warrant a visit to a hospital31,32,33, hospital derived-data does not represent all types of bites and bites that do not


warrant medical attention have been under studied32. Data collected by reviewing veterinary referral cases is also biased to those who are willing to pay for behavioural referral and it is


plausible that this data over-represents large dogs as owners tolerate aggression in smaller dogs for longer34. Surveys and questionnaires regarding being bitten often rely on convenience


sampling, which may lead to a self-selection bias. Detailed interviews with dog bite victims or witnesses of dog bites are an alternative to the above methods26,35, however the sample size


is typically small. Video sharing platforms, such as YouTube, offer an opportunity to address some of the above issues. YouTube has been used to study sequential behaviours and human-dog


interactions within the context in which they occur (e.g. during dog-training) and to collect a more diverse sample of behaviours than veterinary caseload data permits36,37. YouTube provides


a chance to observe the interactions leading to a bite directly, in a naturalistic context. This is important as bite education strategies are often structured around the ladder of


aggression38. This theory proposes that dog behaviours before a bite escalate gradually (in the time immediately before the bite or over the years), with some behaviours (like lip licking or


head turning), being shown earlier in time than other behaviours (like growling or teeth-barring38). This study has the following aims: 1) to summarise the contexts in which dog bites occur


and to describe victim and dog characteristics using YouTube videos of bites, 2) to describe human and dog behaviour preceding a bite, 3) to examine factors that predict the perceived


severity of a bite using variables extracted from YouTube videos, and 4) to evaluate YouTube as a novel method of collecting data about dog bites. METHODS VIDEO SAMPLING Between January 2016


and March 2017, YouTube videos were searched using the following terms: “dog bite”, “dog attack”, “dog bites man/woman”, “dog bites child” and “kid gets bitten”. To increase sample size,


these search terms were translated into Polish and French as the first author speaks these languages. Dog bite was defined as a dog holding a person’s body part in their mouth and applying


pressure, which could be reflected by a bite mark and/or the victim’s vocalisations (e.g. screaming) or facial expressions indicative of pain (e.g. grimacing). We excluded videos under 5 


seconds, that showed bite compilations, ‘bite work’ (i.e. any activity involving teaching a dog to bite or biting on command), or where a bite was not visible (due to quality or content of a


video). All identified videos (N = 653) were watched by the first author and, after excluding duplicates, 143 videos were included in the final sample. This sample was used to describe the


bite context, severity, victim and dog characteristics. Fifty-six videos from this sample showed the behaviour of a dog and a person in detail from the beginning of an interaction until a


bite and were included in analysis of pre-bite behaviour. VIDEO DESCRIPTION Details of victim, dog, context and bite characteristics were extracted from each video (Table 1). Bite severity


is usually approximated by asking if a bite required medical attention or by inspecting the wound39,40. As this was not possible for all videos, we developed a scale, which incorporated


different elements of bites more easily discerned from the videos and expressed ‘perceived severity’. When constructing this measure, the importance of puncture wounds was emphasised,


because bites that result in a puncture have been the basis of previous bite severity scales39,40. We assumed that the puncture did not occur when it was not possible to ascertain whether a


bite broke the skin. Dog head shaking whilst biting was highlighted as it can lead to further lacerations of existing wounds40. The duration of the bite was included as bites that are longer


could be more traumatic. A cut-off point for bite duration was set at one second because most bites observed here were less than that. Where a video showed multiple bites of different


severity, the most extreme scores for variables a, b and c were included to calculate the total score. Perceived severity is defined as (1): $$perceived\,severity={\rm{n}}\,\ast


\,{\rm{a}}+{\rm{b}}+{\rm{c}}$$ (1) where: n = number of bites to the victim observed in the video


\({\rm{a}}=\{\begin{array}{l}{\rm{3}}\,\mbox{--}\,{\rm{puncture}}\,{\rm{wound}}\,{\rm{is}}\,\mathrm{visible}\,\\


{\rm{1}}\,\mbox{--}\,{\rm{puncture}}\,{\rm{wound}}\,{\rm{is}}\,{\rm{not}}\,{\rm{present}}\,{\rm{or}}\,{\rm{not}}\,\mathrm{visible}\,\end{array}\)


\({\rm{b}}=\{\begin{array}{l}{\rm{1}}\,\mbox{--}\,{\rm{dog}}\,{\rm{shook}}\,{\rm{its}}\,{\rm{head}}\,{\rm{whilst}}\,\mathrm{biting}\,\\


{\rm{0}}\,\mbox{--}\,{\rm{dog}}\,{\rm{did}}\,{\rm{not}}\,{\rm{shake}}\,{\rm{its}}\,{\rm{head}}\,{\rm{whilst}}\,\mathrm{biting}\,\end{array}\)


\({\rm{c}}=\{\begin{array}{c}{\rm{1}}\,\mbox{--}\,{\rm{dog}}\,{\rm{held}}\,{\rm{on}}\,{\rm{for}} > \mathrm{1s}\,\\ {\rm{0}}\,\mbox{--}\,{\rm{dog}}\,{\rm{held}}\,{\rm{on}}\,{\rm{for}}\le


\mathrm{1s}\,\end{array}\) To describe the interaction preceding the bite without over-relying on inferring the dog’s motivation or emotions, we adapted a classification used by Reisner _et


al_.24 (Table 2) and each bite was assigned to one context. ETHOGRAM DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOUR CODING Human and dog behaviour ethograms that describe behaviour and movement patterns before


the bite were developed. The dog behaviour ethogram (Supplement 1) was constructed to include behaviours described as preceding a dog bite and often taught as a part of a dog bite prevention


initiatives38, which include displacement activities (such as a shake off, yawning) and appeasement gestures (like head turning or nose licking). In addition, the following behaviours were


included: locomotory behaviours (direction in relation to the person and pace), body, tail and ear posture (as these are associated with negative affect in dogs42), body position,


vocalisation and the type of contact that a dog made with a person (gentle or intensive). To describe human behaviour preceding bites, the following behaviours were included: macro-movements


near the dog (i.e. head and body turns, standing over a dog, moving legs, arms or objects towards the dog), types of tactile contact with a dog, (i.e. petting, hugging, hitting,


restraining, pushing, pulling, holding a body part and lifting), vocalisations, body position and locomotory behaviours (direction in relation to the dog and pace). The descriptions of human


behaviours are based on previous studies exploring human-dog play interactions43 (see Supplement 2 for human behaviour ethogram). We also noted the site of contact on the body and body part


used during contact for both person and a dog. Dogs were coded to make a tactile contact with a person using: head/neck area, mouth, limbs (including tail) and body and a person was coded


to make a contact with a dog using head/neck area, limbs or body. Both dogs and people were coded as being touched on the head/neck area, limbs, or body. The definitions of the behaviours/


behaviour units included in the final ethograms were tested using a sample of the final video dataset, in order to refine the definitions if needed, and to ensure that the behaviours


selected were described in a narrowest possible way, but remained broad enough to be able to identify them across different styles of videos. The videos were coded from beginning of each


clip or a beginning of a human-dog interaction (if a dog and person were not both in the video at the beginning) until the first bite. The ethograms were applied via scan sampling. For this,


the videos were observed for each 3 second interval from the beginning of the video/interaction until the bite and all the behaviours listed in the ethograms which occurred within each 3 


second window were noted. OBSERVER RELIABILITY SCOG and CW, both experienced in analysing dog behaviour, coded a sample of the data independently, compared the results and discussed


discrepancies in classification of the interactions where these occurred to reach a consensus. Subsequently, all videos were coded twice by SCOG (in January and March 2016) and intra-rater


agreement was calculated using Cohen’s kappa. A number of more subjective variables described in Table 1 (dog size, victims’ age, dog breed and bite severity score) were checked for


inter-observer reliability with an observer naïve to the purpose of the study and dog behaviour literature using 10% of randomly selected dog bite videos and the Cohen’s Kappa was


calculated. For both intra- and inter-rater reliability a threshold of 0.61–0.80 was considered acceptable. STATISTICAL ANALYSIS BEHAVIOURS PRECEDING A BITE All statistical analyses were


conducted using R44. To summarize the behavior before the bite, videos across all contexts were pooled and a percentage of occurrence within a given time frame before the bite was provided.


We limited the analysis to 35 seconds before the bite as only 5 videos were longer than this. BITE SEVERITY To understand the association between bite severity score and context, victim and


dog characteristics, we used a hierarchical regression model. The distribution of the bite severity scores was checked and data were assumed gamma distributed, as on visual inspection the


data fit the gamma model better than models for positive integers, e.g. Poisson. Bite severity scores were the dependent variable in these models and were modelled (using a log-link) as a


function of: bite context, the duration of the interaction in seconds, dog size, victim sex, victim age, the anatomical location of the bite, and whether the human or dog initiated the


interaction. The model was hierarchical because varying intercept parameters were included for different bite contexts, and those intercepts were constrained by a common distribution. This


approach reflected that the bite contexts are not completely independent of one another but are a subset of possible categorisations. This allowed partial-pooling of bite severity estimates


across contexts, which often results in more accurate predictions45, particularly when the number of data points per hierarchical group (e.g. the context of a bite) are highly variable46,


which was the case here. For comparison, we also display the sample mean bite severity and 95% confidence interval (CI), derived from non-parametric bootstrapping, for each bite context. To


account for uncertainty in model predictions, the analyses were computed using a Bayesian approach45 utilising Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) with the probabilistic programming language


JAGS version 4.245 through the _runjags_ package in R44. We used model selection to assess whether all of the predictor variables were necessary for predicting bite severity. The baseline


model included the bite contexts, the duration of the interaction and dog size, since these variables were considered _a priori_ important for predicting bite severity. Thirteen additional


models were computed including all combinations of the remaining predictor variables noted above. The best fitting model was recomputed with bite contexts as a fixed effect rather than a


varying effect, to assess whether a hierarchical model was necessary. Models were assessed using the widely applicable information criterion (WAIC), a Bayesian information criterion that


evaluates the out-of-sample predictive accuracy of a model relative to other possible models. Information criteria are preferable to classical measures of model fit (e.g. _R_2) because they


guard against under- and over-fitting to the data46. The WAIC values were transformed to WAIC weights, giving the relative probability of each model having the best out-of-sample predictive


accuracy. Fourteen videos (around 10%) had missing data for who initiated the interaction, so models that included the initiated predictor imputed missing values (see Supplement 3 and


Supplement Table 1 for more details). Prior distributions on regression parameters were broad except for predictor variable coefficients, which had normally distributed priors with means of


0 and standard deviations of 1, further guarding against spurious results in addition to the model selection. The models were run with four MCMC chains, long enough for the effective sample


size for each parameter to be >10,000 and Gelman-Rubin statistics to be \( < \)1.0146. Parameters were summarised by their mean and 95% highest density interval (HDI), the 95% most


probable parameter values (see Supplement 4 for the R script, and Supplement Table 2 for a full dataset). ETHICAL STATEMENT As all videos were in the public domain, ethical approval from the


University Ethics Committee was not required. Videos were used in accordance with YouTube regulations and laws. RESULTS Both intra- and inter- rater agreements on coding were high, and were


considered acceptable (κ = 0.91 and κ = 0.73 respectively). VIDEO CHARACTERISTICS Three hundred and sixty-two bites were observed in 143 videos. Most of the observed dogs were cross-breeds


(n = 47, 32.87%), other common breeds included: Chihuahuas (n = 13, 9.09%), German Shepherds and Pit bulls (n = 11, 7.69% of each) and Labrador Retrievers (n = 6, 4.2%). Almost half of bites


(49.65%) were less than one second long and 74.13% were 3 seconds or less (Supplement Table 2). The victim, dog, bite and context variables are summarized in Table 3. Male victims were more


numerous across all bite contexts and children and infants were more numerous than adults. There were more big dogs compared to medium and small dogs in this sample. The proportion of small


dogs that bit in the context of benign interactions was higher than the proportion of medium- and big-sized dogs (43% vs. 17.95% vs. 20.9%). Victims initiated more interactions than dogs


(48.95% vs. 41.26%). Bites to limbs were more frequent than bites to any other location. The severity score of most bites did not exceed 5, however 51.67% of all bites that scored over 10


points occurred in the context of territorial interactions (Supplement Table 2). BEHAVIOURS PRECEDING A BITE DOG BEHAVIOUR The proportion of videos where dogs were seen holding their body


awkwardly or in a low position and showing a non-neutral ear carriage increased before the bite. The increase in videos where these changes were observed was seen approximately 30 seconds


before the bite for “holding body awkwardly”, 27 seconds before the bite for “body in low position” and 34 seconds before the bite for “non-neutral ear carriage” (Fig. 1). There was no clear


pattern of changes in tail carriage and high body posture before a bite. There was an increase in a proportion of videos where dogs were seen stiffening, snapping and frowning shortly


before the bite (from 22, 16 and 22 seconds before the bite respectively). In the lead up to the bite the proportion of videos where dogs were seen head turning, staring, and panting also


increase (from 28, 30 and 16 seconds before the bite respectively), but these behaviours decreased, plateaued or fluctuated shortly before the bite. Yawning and shake off were observed


sporadically and lip licking, paw raises and sniffing did not follow any clear pattern (Fig. 1). There was an increase in the proportion of dogs growling and a decrease in dogs being silent


or barking before the bite. Pain-related vocalisations were rare. Closer in time to the bite, more dogs were coded as restrained and fewer were coded as standing. There was no clear pattern


regarding play bows, sitting and laying down. As the bite became closer, there was more of fast pace locomotory behaviours and less jumping and slow pace locomotory behaviours. There was no


clear pattern regarding dogs making a gentle contact before the bite and there was a clear spike in a proportion of dogs making an intensive contact immediately before the bite, which


reflects the moment of a bite. Dogs touched people mainly with their paws and there was no pattern in tactile contact initiated by the dog before a bite (Supplement Table 2). HUMAN BEHAVIOUR


Behaviours grouped as ‘movements without contact with a dog’ were observed more than 50% of the time. Leading to a bite, there was an increase in codes representing standing over a dog


(approximately 35 seconds before the bite, Fig. 2). There was no clear pattern to all other non-contact behaviours. All of the contact behaviours increased approximately 21 seconds before


the bite with petting and restraining a dog being particularly frequent (Fig. 2). Hugging, hitting, pushing and pulling did not follow any clear pattern. Kissing, hitting with an object,


kicking and pulling hair were not observed or were rare. Until 21 seconds before the bite there were proportionally more codes for movement towards the dog and from 9 seconds before the bite


more codes for movement away from the dog were noted. There was no clear trend regarding changes of pace of movement in time before the bite. There was a sharp increase in the proportion of


pain-related vocalisations immediately before the bite (which could indicate anticipation of pain rather than the experience of pain) and a slight increase in laughing vocalisations from 15


 seconds before the bite. Normal talk and silence were observed proportionally less often closer in time to the bite. Shortly before the bite standing and crouching behaviours decreased and


there were slightly more laying down behaviours (from 18 seconds before the bite). People usually made a tactile contact with a dog using limbs and there was no clear pattern regarding the


part of the dog that was touched (Supplement Table 2). MODEL SELECTION Three models had demonstrably lower WAIC values and higher WAIC weights than the other models assessed (see Supplement 


5 for model comparison). The model including varying effects for bite contexts and all predictor variables was ranked first, with WAIC weight of 45%. The same model with bite contexts


included as a fixed rather than a varying effect had a WAIC weight of 20%. The model with varying effects for bite contexts, interaction duration, dog size, the anatomical location of the


bite and whether the human or dog initiated the interaction and where victim’s sex and age were excluded, had a WAIC weight of 33%. Thus, all predictors appeared important to predicting


severity. BITE CONTEXTS Across bite contexts, the mean bite severity score was estimated as 5.61 (95% HDI: 4.16, 7.27, see Fig. 2 and Supplement Table 2). Due to the varying numbers of


videos in each category, differences among contexts were pooled closer to the overall mean compared to the raw data. The _benign_ and _play_ contexts have the most influence due to having


the largest sample sizes. Moreover, the _territorial_ context appeared to have a larger mean estimate but it only had 14 videos with large variation, as evidenced by its 95% CI, resulting in


its estimate being pooled towards the overall mean. The intra-class correlation coefficient was both highly uncertain and included zero (mean = 0.11; 95% HDI: 0.00, 0.38), suggesting


greater within- than between-context variation for bite severity. The 95% HDI estimates for differences between bite contexts all overlapped zero (see Supplement 6), i.e. we did not observe


any significant difference in bite severity between different contexts of interactions (Fig. 3), however, bites in the public space and territorial contexts and painful interactions were


more severe than the mean, whereas bites in the context of resting were less severe than the mean, when taking into account just sample mean and bootstrap 95% CI. OTHER PREDICTOR VARIABLES


Among the fixed-effect predictor variables, bite severity scores increased by an average of 1.25 points with approximately every minute (one SD = 56 s) increase in the duration of the


interaction (see Supplement 7). The 95% HDI of this estimate extended from 1.09 to 1.41, indicating it was significant. This is somewhat expected as longer videos/longer interactions could


feature more bites, which is a variable used for calculating bite severity. Videos with bites to multiple locations had significantly higher bite severity scores than videos with bites only


to the limbs (\({\beta }_{{limbs}-{multiple}}\) = −6.82; 95% HDI: −12.15, −2.31; Supplement 6) or only to the face (\({\beta }_{{face}-{multiple}}\) = −7.76; 95% HDI: −13.37, −2.62), which


again could be due to more bites being observed meaning greater severity score. When the victim was an adult, bites were more severe than when the victim was a child (\({\beta


}_{{child}-{adult}}\) = −1.61; 95% HDI: −3.16, −0.08) and bites to infants were more severe than those to children (\({\beta }_{{infant}-{child}}\) = 2.17; 95% HDI: 0.14, 4.37). All other


predictor variables had 95% HDI estimates overlapping zero, meaning that they were not significant (Fig. 4; Supplement 6). DISCUSSION In this study, we used YouTube videos to explore dog


bites to humans. The most common breeds and types of dogs found in our sample (i.e. German Shepherds, Chihuahuas, Labrador Retrievers and Pit bulls) reflect those previously identified in


studies of dog bites10,11,12,13,15,16,17,47. Chihuahuas are rarely mentioned in studies that use hospital admissions, possibly because their small size makes them less likely to cause


serious injury. However, a study using a Canine Behavioural Assessment and Research Questionnaire (a validated questionnaire for assessing dogs’ behaviour) compared 30 breeds for aggressive


behaviour and found that Chihuahuas were higher than average for human- and dog- directed aggression48. In addition to this, we hypothesize that bites by a small dog may be perceived as


comical and thus more often uploaded online. It is also unclear if the breeds observed here are more likely to bite or to be more commonly owned13,15,23. In our study, male victims were


over-represented. This trend has been noticed in previous publications1,5,12,13,15 but not to the same extent. It is therefore plausible that clips showing men are more often shared online.


Our study included a similar proportion of adults to children and infants as those previously reported12,16,17,49, with children and infants being considerably more common victims than


adults. Here, most bites were to the limbs, followed by bites to the face and neck area. Bites to face and neck area were more common among children and infants, which is also consistent


with earlier reports1,10,11,27. A variety of bite contexts were represented in our sample. Bites during play and benign interactions were particularly common, as reported


before10,14,16,18,24,50. Play bites as well as behaviours that we labelled as benign interactions may be perceived as a normal part of human-dog behavioural repertoires and thus more


frequently permitted and easier to film than other bite contexts. Moreover, bite victims may not see all bites as ‘true’ bites35, which could lead to bites in contexts such as play being


more often uploaded online. In contrast to Reisner _et al_.24, we found that bites in the context of resources and resting were rare. The dissimilarity could arise due to differences in


studied population: our sample consisted of all age groups, whilst Reisner _et al_.24 included only children. Alternatively, it could be due to these contexts being unlikely to be filmed.


Here we followed the classification used by Reisner _et al_.24 to aide comparison and because it permits categorisation based on the observed behaviours that can easily be recognised by an


untrained observer. This terminology can, however, be misleading. For instance, previous research suggests that ‘benign’ interactions, as perceived by victims, may not be pleasant for dogs.


Dogs may dislike being petted on top of their heads49,51, although we did not see a clear increase in tactile contact with head and neck areas before the bite. Displacement and appeasement


behaviours as well as postural changes and vocalisations were included in this analysis as they are often discussed as preceding a bite and taught as a part of bite-prevention education38.


Closer to the time of the bite, dogs were more often coded as holding their body low or in an awkward position and their ears were more often observed to be in a non-neutral position. The


postural changes have been linked with dogs experiencing acute distress in response to a fear-inducing stimuli42 and changes in ear carriage have been observed during training that involved


painful stimuli52. It is plausible that these changes were detected here as some interactions leading to a bite may be painful or cause a distress to a dog. However, not all dogs in the


videos show these changes and we also did not observe any clear changes in tail carriage pattern. Nonetheless, as postural changes may be easier to spot than some of the more subtle distance


increasing behaviours, and as an increase in these behaviours was observed from approximately 30 seconds before the bite, bite-prevention messages should emphasise them more. Following from


the ladder of aggression theory38, behaviours such as lip licking, head turning are expected to escalate and be replaced with behaviours like snapping or growling in time before the bite.


Head turning and full body turning as well as staring, stiffening, snapping, growling and frowning were observed proportionally more often in a build up to a bite, with head turning and


staring dropping immediately before the bite, as would be expected from the ladder of aggression theory. We observed an increase in these behaviours approximately 20 seconds before the bite,


which suggests that a person interacting with a dog does have time to alter their behaviour in response to these signs. However, as the increase in these behaviours is gradual, a person may


not recognise their presence until later, if at all. A person may also recognise these signs and carry on interactions, assuming that a bite “would not happen to them”26. Other behaviours


included in the ladder of aggression (like lip licking, paw lifting and sniffing) did not follow a clear pattern and sniffing and paw lifts were rarely observed. However, these behaviours


may have escalated over a longer period of time, for instance a dog may have shown some of the behaviours from the lower steps of the ladder during previous interactions with a person, which


would not have been captured in the video studied. Alternatively, these behaviours may not fit the pattern of behaviour progression proposed by the ladder of aggression theory. Overall, the


postural changes were observed more often than other behaviours included in the ladder of aggression. Previous studies linked some of these behaviours (lip licking, paw lifts, head turns


and yawning) with acute stress and pain42,53, emotional conflict52 and as a response to human facial expressions linked with a negative emotional valence54 which may be specific to some, but


not all contexts in which bites occur. Standing over a dog, petting and restraining a dog were seen proportionally more frequently closer to the bite, increasing approximately 20–30 seconds


before. Other behaviours that did not result in contact and other tactile behaviours did not follow any clear pattern. The high prevalence of ‘standing over’ codes in the time preceding a


bite suggests this particular behaviour should be emphasised in bite prevention training. The high frequency of petting and restraining behaviours makes prevention advice challenging, as


these types of contacts are likely to occur when a person is familiar with the dog and interacts with a dog on daily basis, in a routine, habitual fashion. This results shows that dog owner


education should emphasise the idea of all interactions with a dog, and in particular tactile interactions like petting, should be mutually consensual, i.e. only initiated by a person after


a dog has already made a contact or otherwise shown an interest in being petted. In addition, restraining a dog e.g. in order to medicate it or prevent it from escaping may be hard to avoid


and therefore requires additional care. It indicates the importance of teaching low-stress handling methods55. The regression model with mean and 95% HDI estimates identified no significant


differences in bite severity between bite contexts. This could be because there may be more similarities between bite contexts than differences, making the distinction between contexts


difficult. However, the analysis of sample mean and bootstrap 95% CI suggested that territorial bites and bites in public spaces were more severe than other bites. Bites in the context of


benign and unpleasant interactions and resting were less severe, which reflects previous research27. The bootstrap analysis also indicated that when a dog initiated the interactions (vs. a


person), bite severity was greater. Bites in the context of benign interactions and unpleasant interactions may be more inhibited as the victim involved is likely to be more familiar with


the dog. It is also plausible that the dog-initiated interactions in general may include more offensive aggression, whereas the human-initiated interactions may reflect more of the defensive


aggression56. Different motivation to aggress could explain differences in severity as, in cases of defensive aggression, a dog may strive to warn off, which may result in a lesser severity


of bite. Overall, other predictor variables – and, in particular, the location of the bite on the body, age of the victim and size of the dog – were better at predicting the severity of a


bite than context alone, although even here, the regression model with mean and 95% HDI estimates were not always significant. Again, this could be due to a small sample size or the way the


severity measure was derived. In general, there may be numerous interactive effects between predictor variables that were not possible to explore in this study due to limited sample size and


differences in number of videos in each context. The analysis showed that severity of a bite was correlated with the duration of a video, regardless of the context of interactions or other


predictor variables. This could be because a person who was attacked for longer received more serious injuries or because longer attacks simply score higher severity mark. Moreover, not all


variables which are often cited in literature as risk factors for bites were used as predictor variables in our model as it was not always possible to discern them from the video. We also


did not include a breed as a predictor variable due to documented problems in recognising a breed based on visual characteristics30 and a small number of dogs in each breed category. One


approach that could be utilised in the future on larger samples is systems-style modelling, such as using network analysis, that would identify interactive effects between variables that


result in bites occurring and/or influence bite severity. Using YouTube to study dog bites enabled us to carry out observations of bites of diverse severity, in naturalistic settings and


across a range of contexts. The benefit of this approach is that permits studying human and dog behaviours preceding bites, which is not possible with other retrospective methods. However,


the sample generated through YouTube search is subject to some biases as the frequency of bites in a given context and the victim and dog characteristics could reflect the likelihood with


which these interactions are filmed and the self-selection bias for uploading videos online. The quality of videos and editing styles varied across the sample which meant that we could not


collect a fine level of detail from each video. Small sample size meant that the analysis of body language had to be restricted to simple descriptive statistics, which is a further


limitation of this method. Moreover, our analysis is limited because the bite severity score reflects the perception of severity as observed in the video, as it is impossible to assess the


full extent of each injury. It is plausible that as puncture wounds may be more difficult to identify in some videos, we sometimes under-estimated the severity. The same score on severity


could, for example, reflect different elements of the bite; videos in the context of play may have a high severity score due to number of bites in a video whereas bites in the context of


territorial aggression could have a high score due to puncture wounds and tearing movement of the dog, whereas in the reality, the later would cause more damage. CONCLUSION In summary, this


study used a novel approach to analyse human-dog interactions in naturalistic contexts. We found that despite potential biases of this sample, the demographic characteristics of the victims


and dogs seen in YouTube bite videos reflect those found in previous publications. Although our analysis did not allow exploration of the causal relationship between human behaviour and dog


bites, we observed that tactile contact with a dog increases approximately 20 seconds before a bite as does standing or leaning over a dog. Prevention messages could emphasise the risk of


leaning over a dog and simply advise avoiding contact with a dog when possible or in doubt (for instance, when interacting with an unfamiliar dog). In a lead up to a bite, changes in dog


body posture were more obvious than changes in appeasement and displacement behaviours. Some, but not all, appeasement and displacement behaviours described in the “ladder of aggression”38


were also observed. REFERENCES * Winter, J. Admission caused by dogs and other mammals,


https://digital.nhs.uk/media/33701/Provisional-Monthly-HES-for-Admitted-Patient-Care-Outpatient-and-Accident-and-Emergency-Data-April-2014-to-February-2015-Topic-of-Interest-Admissions-caused-by-dogs-and-other-mammals/pdf/Animal_Bites_M12_1415


(2015). * Anon. NHS Hospital Stay, https://digital.nhs.uk/catalogue/PUB19124 (2015). * Benson, L. S., Edwards, S. L., Schiff, A. P., Williams, C. S. & Visotsky, J. L. Dog and cat bites


to the hand: treatment and cost assessment. _The Journal of hand surgery_ 31, 468–473 (2006). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Peters, V., Sottiaux, M., Appelboom, J. & Kahn, A.


Posttraumatic stress disorder after dog bites in children. _The Journal of pediatrics_ 144, 121–122 (2004). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Overall, K. L. & Love, M. Dog bites to


humans-demography, epidemiology, injury, and risk. _Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association_ 218, 1923–1934 (2001). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Abuabara, A. A


review of facial injuries due to dog bites. _Medicina Oral, Patología Oral y Cirugía Bucal (Internet)_ 11, 348–350 (2006). Google Scholar  * Knobel, D. L. _et al_. Re-evaluating the burden


of rabies in Africa and Asia. _Bulletin of the World health Organization_ 83, 360–368 (2005). PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Kass, P. H., New, J. J. C., Scarlett, J. M. &


Salman, M. D. Understanding Animal Companion Surplus in the United States: Relinquishment of Nonadoptables to Animal Shelters for Euthanasia. _Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science_ 4,


237–248 (2001). Article  Google Scholar  * BVA. (2016). * Touré, G., Angoulangouli, G. & Méningaud, J.-P. Epidemiology and classification of dog bite injuries to the face: A prospective


study of 108 patients. _Journal of Plastic, Reconstructive & Aesthetic Surgery_ 68, 654–658 (2015). Article  Google Scholar  * Súilleabháin, P. Ó. Human hospitalisations due to dog bites


in Ireland (1998–2013): Implications for current breed specific legislation. _The Veterinary Journal_ 204, 357–359 (2015). Article  Google Scholar  * Klaassen, B., Buckley, J. & Esmail,


A. Does the dangerous dogs act protect against animal attacks: a prospective study of mammalian bites in the accident and emergency department. _Injury_ 27, 89–91 (1996). Article  CAS 


PubMed  Google Scholar  * Rezac, P., Rezac, K. & Slama, P. Human behavior preceding dog bites to the face. _The Veterinary Journal_ 206, 284–288 (2015). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google


Scholar  * Mannion, C., Graham, A., Shepherd, K. & Greenberg, D. Dog bites and maxillofacial surgery: what can we do? _British journal of oral and maxillofacial surgery_ 53, 522–525


(2015). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Cornelissen, J. M. & Hopster, H. Dog bites in The Netherlands: A study of victims, injuries, circumstances and aggressors to support


evaluation of breed specific legislation. _The Veterinary Journal_ 186, 292–298 (2010). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Rosado, B., García-Belenguer, S., León, M. & Palacio, J.


Spanish dangerous animals act: Effect on the epidemiology of dog bites. _Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research_ 2, 166–174 (2007). Article  Google Scholar  *


Guy, N. C. _et al_. Risk factors for dog bites to owners in a general veterinary caseload. _Applied Animal Behaviour Science_ 74, 29–42 (2001). Article  Google Scholar  * Reisner, I. R. _et


al_. Behavioural characteristics associated with dog bites to children presenting to an urban trauma centre. _Injury prevention, ip._ 2010, 029868 (2011). Google Scholar  * Meints, K.,


Syrnyk, C. & De Keuster, T. Why do children get bitten in the face? _Injury Prevention_ 16, A172–A173 (2010). Article  Google Scholar  * Garvey, E. M., Twitchell, D. K., Ragar, R., Egan,


J. C. & Jamshidi, R. Morbidity of pediatric dog bites: A case series at a level one pediatric trauma center. _Journal of pediatric surgery_ 50, 343–346 (2015). Article  PubMed  Google


Scholar  * O’Brien, D. C., Andre, T. B., Robinson, A. D., Squires, L. D. & Tollefson, T. T. Dog bites of the head and neck: an evaluation of a common pediatric trauma and associated


treatment. _American journal of otolaryngology_ 36, 32–38 (2015). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Pédrono, G. _et al_. 483 Dog bites: severity and sequelae, a multicenter survey, France,


September 2010–December 2011. _Injury Prevention_ 22, A175–A175 (2016). Google Scholar  * Ó Súilleabháin, P. & Doherty, N. Epidemiology of dog bite injuries: Dog-breed identification and


dog-owner interaction. _Journal of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery_ 68, 1157–1158, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjps.2015.03.025 (2015). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  *


Reisner, I. R., Shofer, F. S. & Nance, M. L. Behavioral assessment of child-directed canine aggression. _Injury Prevention_ 13, 348–351 (2007). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google


Scholar  * Náhlík, J., Baranyiová, E. & Tyrlík, M. Dog Bites to Children in the Czech Republic: the Risk Situations. _Acta Veterinaria Brno_ 79, 627–636 (2011). Article  Google Scholar 


* Westgarth, C. & Watkins, F. A qualitative investigation of the perceptions of female dog-bite victims and implications for the prevention of dog bites. _Journal of veterinary behavior:


Clinical applications and research_ 10, 479–488 (2015). Article  Google Scholar  * Shuler, C. M., DeBess, E. E., Lapidus, J. A. & Hedberg, K. Canine and human factors related to dog


bite injuries. _Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association_ 232, 542–546 (2008). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Casey, R. A., Loftus, B., Bolster, C., Richards, G. J. &


Blackwell, E. J. Human directed aggression in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Occurrence in different contexts and risk factors. _Applied Animal Behaviour Science_ 152, 52–63 (2014).


Article  Google Scholar  * Bernardo, L. M. _et al_. Documentation of Growls and Bites in the Emergency Setting. _Journal of Emergency Nursing_ 28, 5 (2002). Article  Google Scholar  *


Westgarth, C. Is _that dog a pit bull? A cross-country comparison of perceptions of shelter workers regarding breed identificatio_n. (2014). * Sacks, J. J., Kresnow, M.-J. & Houston, B.


Dog bites: how big a problem? _Injury prevention_ 2, 52–54 (1996). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Gilchrist, J., Sacks, J., White, D. & Kresnow, M. Dog bites:


still a problem? _Injury prevention_ 14, 296–301 (2008). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Westgarth, C., Brooke, M. & Christley, R. M. How many people have been bitten by dogs? A


cross-sectional survey of prevalence, incidence and factors associated with dog bites in a UK community. _Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health_ 72, 331–336,


https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2017-209330 (2018). PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Arhant, C., Landenberger, R., Beetz, A. & Troxler, J. Attitudes of caregivers to supervision of


child–family dog interactions in children up to 6 years—An exploratory study. _Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research_ 14, 10–16 (2016). Article  Google Scholar


  * Orritt, R., Gross, H. & Hogue, T. His bark is worse than his bite: perceptions and rationalization of canine aggressive behavior. _Human-Animal Interaction Bulletin_ 3, 1–20 (2015).


Google Scholar  * Preston, S. M., Shihab, N. & Volk, H. A. Public perception of epilepsy in dogs is more favorable than in humans. _Epilepsy & Behavior_ 27, 243–246 (2013). Article 


Google Scholar  * Burn, C. C. A vicious cycle: a cross-sectional study of canine tail-chasing and human responses to it, using a free video-sharing website. _PloS one_ 6, e26553 (2011).


Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Shepherd, K. Ladder of aggression. _BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Behavioural Medicine_, 13–16 (2009). * Dunbar, I. The Bite


Scale. An Objective Assessment of the Severity of Dog Bites Based on Evaluation of Wound Pathology, http://www.dogtalk.com/BiteAssessmentScalesDunbarDTMRoss.pdf (ND). * Lackmann, G.-M.,


Draf, W., Isselstein, G. & Töllner, U. Surgical treatment of facial dog bite injuries in children. _Journal of Cranio-Maxillofacial Surgery_ 20, 81–86 (1992). Article  CAS  PubMed 


Google Scholar  * Anon. Breed Information Centre http://www.thekennelclub.org.uk/services/public/breed/ (2016). * Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B., van Hooff, J. A., de Vries, H. W. & Mol, J.


A. Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. _Applied Animal Behaviour Science_ 58, 365–381 (1998). Article  Google Scholar  * Rooney, N.


J., Bradshaw, J. W. & Robinson, I. H. Do dogs respond to play signals given by humans? _Animal Behaviour_ 61, 715–722 (2001). Article  Google Scholar  * Statistical Package, R. R: A


language and environment for statistical computing. _Vienna_, _Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing_ (2009). * Kruschke, J. _Doing Bayesian data analysis: A tutorial with R_,


_JAGS_, _and Stan_. (Academic Press, 2014). * Gelman, A., Gelman, A. & Hill, J. _Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models_. (Cambridge; Cambridge University


Press, 2007). * Wake, A., Minot, E., Stafford, K. & Perry, P. A survey of adult victims of dog bites in New Zealand. _New Zealand veterinary journal_ 57, 364–369 (2009). Article  CAS 


PubMed  Google Scholar  * Duffy, D. L., Hsu, Y. & Serpell, J. A. Breed differences in canine aggression. _Applied Animal Behaviour Science_ 114, 441–460 (2008). Article  Google Scholar 


* De Keuster, T., Lamoureux, J. & Kahn, A. Epidemiology of dog bites: a Belgian experience of canine behaviour and public health concerns. _The Veterinary Journal_ 172, 482–487 (2006).


Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Mannion, C. & Mills, D. Injuries sustained by dog bites. _The British journal of oral & maxillofacial surgery_ 51, 368–369 (2013). Article  CAS 


Google Scholar  * Kuhne, F., Hößler, J. C. & Struwe, R. Behavioral and cardiac responses by dogs to physical human–dog contact. _Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and


Research_ 9, 93–97 (2014). Article  Google Scholar  * Schilder, M. B. & van der Borg, J. A. Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short and long term behavioural effects.


_Applied Animal Behaviour Science_ 85, 319–334 (2004). Article  Google Scholar  * Hekman, J. P., Karas, A. Z. & Dreschel, N. A. Salivary cortisol concentrations and behavior in a


population of healthy dogs hospitalized for elective procedures. _Applied animal behaviour science_ 141, 149–157 (2012). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Albuquerque, N.,


Guo, K., Wilkinson, A., Resende, B. & Mills, D. S. Mouth-licking by dogs as a response to emotional stimuli. _Behavioural processes_ 146, 42–45 (2018). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  *


Yin, S. A. _Low stress handling_, _restraint and behavior modification of dogs & cats_. (CattleDog Pub., 2009). * Fatjo, J., Amat, M., Mariotti, V. M., de la Torre, J. L. R. &


Manteca, X. Analysis of 1040 cases of canine aggression in a referral practice in Spain. _Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research_ 2, 158–165 (2007). Article 


Google Scholar  Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The authors are grateful to the funders for their support and to Zuzanna Walczak for coding 10% videos for the inter-rater reliability


analysis and Chloe O’Sullivan for help in ethogram development. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, Institute of Infection and


Global Health, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, L69 7BE, UK Sara C. Owczarczak-Garstecka, Rob Christley & Carri Westgarth * Institute for Risk and Uncertainty, University of


Liverpool, Liverpool, L69 7ZF, UK Sara C. Owczarczak-Garstecka & Carri Westgarth * Department of Public Health and Policy, Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, University of


Liverpool, Liverpool, L69 3GL, UK Francine Watkins Authors * Sara C. Owczarczak-Garstecka View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Francine


Watkins View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Rob Christley View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google


Scholar * Carri Westgarth View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS S.C.O.G. coded the videos, S.C.O.G., C.W., F.W. and R.C. wrote


the main manuscript text, S.C.O.G. and R.C. prepared the figures and S.C.O.G. carried out the analysis. All authors reviewed the manuscript. CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Sara C.


Owczarczak-Garstecka. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS SCOG PhD studentship is funded by the EPSRC and ESRC research councils, Institute for Risk and Uncertainty and Dogs Trust. The


funders had no role in the conceptualization, design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. SCOG, RC and CW have been bitten by dogs. ADDITIONAL


INFORMATION PUBLISHER'S NOTE: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL


SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION FILE SUPPLEMENTARY DATASET 1 SUPPLEMENTARY DATASET 2 RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0


International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the


source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative


Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by


statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit


http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Owczarczak-Garstecka, S.C., Watkins, F., Christley, R. _et al._ Online videos


indicate human and dog behaviour preceding dog bites and the context in which bites occur. _Sci Rep_ 8, 7147 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25671-7 Download citation * Received:


03 January 2018 * Accepted: 26 April 2018 * Published: 08 May 2018 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25671-7 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able


to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing


initiative