Why do people seek out polygenic risk scores for complex disorders, and how do they understand and react to results?

Why do people seek out polygenic risk scores for complex disorders, and how do they understand and react to results?


Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT We sought to explore individuals’ motivations for using their direct-to-consumer genetic testing data to generate polygenic risk scores (PRSs) using a not-for-profit third-party


tool, and to assess understanding of, and reaction to their results. Using a cross-sectional design, users of Impute.me who had already accessed PRS results were invited to complete an


online questionnaire asking about demographics, motivations for seeking PRSs, understanding and interpretation of PRSs, and two validated scales regarding reactions to results—the Impact of


Event Scale Revised (IES-R) and the Feelings About genomiC Testing Results (FACToR). Independent samples _T_-tests and ANOVA were used to explore associations between the variables. 227


individuals participated in the study. The most frequently reported motivation was general curiosity (98.2%). Only 25.6% of participants correctly answered all questions assessing


understanding/interpretation of PRSs. Over half of participants (60.8%) experienced a negative reaction (upset, anxious, and/or sad on FACToR scale) after receiving their PRSs and 5.3%


scored over the threshold for potential post-traumatic stress disorder on the IES-R. Lower understanding about PRS was associated with experiencing a negative psychological reaction (_P_


values <0.001). Higher quality pre-test information, particularly to improve understanding, and manage expectations for PRS may be useful in limiting negative psychological reactions. You


have full access to this article via your institution. Download PDF SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS RISK PERCEPTION AND INTENDED BEHAVIOR CHANGE AFTER UNINFORMATIVE GENETIC RESULTS


FOR ADULT-ONSET HEREDITARY CONDITIONS IN UNSELECTED PATIENTS Article 26 September 2023 WHAT PEOPLE REALLY CHANGE AFTER GENETIC TESTING (GT) PERFORMED IN PRIVATE LABS: RESULTS FROM AN ITALIAN


STUDY Article Open access 12 April 2021 KNOWLEDGE, ATTITUDES AND DEMAND TOWARD CARDIOVASCULAR POLYGENIC RISK TESTING IN CLINICAL PRACTICE: CROSS-SECTIONAL SURVEY OF PATIENTS Article 07


December 2024 INTRODUCTION Most common conditions that affect humans are etiologically complex, with the genetic risk being conferred by an accumulation of many genetic variants of small


effect [1]. These variants can be summarized into a single variable, a polygenic risk score (PRS), that measures the genetic contribution for a given trait or condition. While for most


disorders PRSs are not yet being used in clinical practice [2], they are likely to become a more routine part of clinical care in the future [1]. However in the meantime, public interest in


genetic information has led companies that already provide direct-to-consumer (DTC) genetic testing online [3,4,5] to include PRSs in their offerings. Third-party services have also emerged


which allow users to upload their raw personal genetic information obtained from DTC sites (like 23andMe or Ancestry) and generate PRSs for a wide variety of conditions [6]. Emerging data


demonstrate very rapid increase in the usage of these third-party sites [6]. Despite the rapid growth in rate with which people are accessing their own PRSs without the involvement of a


healthcare provider [6], little is known about what motivates people to seek this information, or how they understand and react to the results that they receive. Historically, patient


outcomes of genetic testing have been found to be generally benign, however, most studies have focused on individuals who were known to be at high-risk for a monogenic disease based on


personal or family history [7, 8]. Importantly, the majority of studies assessing the emotional impacts of genetic testing have been conducted in a clinical context that was accompanied by


genetic counseling [7, 8]. There have been studies which have focused on outcomes of DTC genetic testing, although the majority have not explored the experiences of actual consumers and


instead used hypothetical scenarios or provided free ‘DTC’ genetic testing for recruited participants [9,10,11]. A number of these studies have suggested the potential for slight negative


reactions, however, current research that has examined actual reactions to DTC tests is limited and remains less clear [11]. There is a small but growing body of literature about


individuals’ reactions to receiving PRSs, however these studies have predominantly focused on recruiting people to receive PRSs for specific conditions (e.g., cancer) for which the


participants already had increased risk due to personal or family history and the PRS results were delivered in the context of carefully designed interventions [12,13,14,15]. To our


knowledge, only one study has explored the impact of a variety of PRSs on individuals who were not specifically at risk for a particular condition. In this study, participants were recruited


from the staff of health and technology companies, and education, informed consent, and genetic counseling about PRSs accompanied the testing [16]. There is very little known about the


experiences of our population of interest, where individuals are self-initiating the process of seeking out PRSs and uploading their DTC genetic testing data into third-party websites to


access this information for themselves. We sought to elicit motivations for seeking PRSs, understanding of the results, and psychological reactions to receiving this information among


individuals who had sought out PRSs for themselves by uploading their own raw genetic testing data into a third-party PRS website. MATERIALS AND METHODS RECRUITMENT Participants were


recruited from users of Impute.me; a third-party non-profit tool that provides PRSs for 166 different conditions from personalized raw genetic data (e.g., generated by 23andMe) to users at


no cost. The website (https://www.impute.me) was established in 2015 and is run by independent academics. Impute.me has strict privacy standards; all personally traceable and non-traceable


genetic information is deleted two weeks and two years after imputation, respectively [6]. Users upload their raw genetic data to Impute.me and access the complex disorders module on the


website to receive polygenetic risk scores for a variety of traits and conditions. The user chooses which conditions to query and after performing five or more queries on the website’s


complex disorders module with their personal results, users were invited to complete a questionnaire on the REDCap [17] platform via a pop-up banner containing a link. Individuals were


eligible to participate if they: (1) had received PRS results from the Impute.me website for at least five complex disorders and were (2) at least 18 years of age, and (3) fluent in English.


The study was launched in June 2019 and data collection ended in April 2020. The study was approved by the University of British Columbia’s Research Ethics Board (H19-00427). For more


information on how PRS are provided by Impute.me, we have included sample reports in the Supplementary Information (Supplementary Figs. 1 and 2). QUESTIONNAIRE The questionnaire contained


three main sections: users’ motivations for seeking PRSs, their understanding and interpretation of the results, and the psychological impacts of receiving this information (see


Supplementary materials). The survey also included demographic items (age, sex, gender, ethnicity, education level, household income, history of psychiatric illness, and the time elapsed


since participants received their results from Impute.me, see Table 1) and a three-item numeracy scale previously found to have acceptable reliability [18]. The survey did not collect or


link responses to the participants’ PRS data. MOTIVATIONS To explore motivations for seeking PRS information, participants were asked to rate ten items (see Fig. 1) on a four-point Likert


scale ranging from “this was NOT part of the reason for me to upload my data to Impute.me” to “this was the MAIN reason for me to upload my data to Impute.me”. During data analysis the


middle categories were combined to form a new category: “PART of the reason”. Participants were provided a list of the 58 most inquired-about conditions (those reviewed by ≥25% of all users)


and asked to select those for which they were initially interested in learning about their risk. Because individual PRS results were not linked to the survey responses, we used this


question to explore if there were specific conditions that motivated interest in PRSs. There were also two free text items regarding initial motivations. UNDERSTANDING AND INTERPRETATION OF


RESULTS Participants’ _understanding_ of the meaning of the PRS results was explored with three true/false statements (see Table 2), and to assess _interpretation_ of PRS result information,


participants were provided an example Impute.me PRS result, and asked to answer five true/false statements. REACTIONS We used two established scales to explore psychological reactions to


PRSs, the Feelings About genomiC Testing Results (FACToR) questionnaire, and the Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES-R). Wording for both instructed participants to use the receipt of the PRS


results as the reference point for answering the items. The FACToR scale is a 12 item, preliminarily validated [19] tool adapted from the MICRA scale [20] that contains four subscales:


Negative Emotions, Positive Feelings, Uncertainty and Privacy Concerns. Summary scores for each subscale are calculated by adding scores from individual items in that subscale, with possible


scores ranging from 0-12 for the Negative Emotions and Uncertainty Subscales, 0-16 on the Positive Feelings Subscale, and 0-8 on the Privacy Concerns Subscale. For items that measure


positive feelings, scores were first reversed before being summed, such that for all scales, a higher score indicates greater functional impairment [19]. We focused on the negative emotions


subscale because understanding factors that contribute to patient harm has the greatest likelihood to impact clinical practice around the delivery of PRS information. The IES-R is a 22 item


validated instrument [21] that assesses subjective distress associated with events. IES-R scale scores range from 0 to 88, with a score >23 indicating likely clinically significant


distress, scores between 33 and 38 suggesting a probable diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and scores >38 indicating significant PTSD [21]. DATA ANALYSIS Analysis was


primarily descriptive, but given the clinical importance of negative reactions to PRS, we used exploratory statistical tests to examine relationships between these reactions and other


variables. Specifically, we explored relationships between the IES-R and FACToR Negative Emotions Subscale scores and demographic variables using independent samples _t_ tests and ANOVA,


with post-hoc Tukey to further explore significant relationships. We also compared IES-R and FACToR Negative Emotions Subscale scores between those who did/did not: (a) chose “curiosity” as


a main reason for seeking their PRS, (b) answer all of the numeracy questions correctly, (c) answer all three of the understanding questions correctly, and d) answer all five of the


interpretation questions correctly (independent sample _t_ tests). As well, we explored relationships between the number of motivators participants selected and, first, IES-R (those who


scored above and below cutoff for PTSD), and second, FACToR Negative Emotions Subscale score (independent samples _t_ tests, and Pearson correlation/linear regression respectively). All


statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 23 (IBM Corp., Armonk, New York). Although this was an exploratory study, we used a conservative approach, and set our


threshold for statistical significance at _p_ < 0.002 (Bonferroni correction accounting for the 26 tests we conducted). Text that participants provided in the free-form text fields was


used to provide illustrative quotes around key findings. RESULTS Of the 10,650 people who were eligible to participate, 438 (4.1%) clicked on the link for more information and 277


participated in the study (2.6% of eligible). Respondents tended to be female (_n_ = 144, 63.7%), White (_n_ = 200, 88.5%), with high levels of education (_n_ = 121, 53.3 % had a bachelor’s


or graduate degree), and most (_n_ = 162, 71.7%) had received their results for the first time within the week prior to completing the questionnaire (see Table 1). Incomplete surveys were


included in analysis and therefore response rate varied by question. MOTIVATIONS Almost all participants indicated that general curiosity (_n_ = 270, 98.2%) and accessing additional health


information (_n_ = 265, 96.0%) were at least part of their reason for seeking PRSs (see Fig. 1). The conditions that were most frequently identified by participants as being of interest


included Alzheimer’s disease (_n_ = 127, 50.0%), anxiety disorder (_n_ = 105, 41.3%), major depressive disorder (_n_ = 98, 38.6%), type 2 diabetes (_n_ = 97, 38.2%), and breast cancer (_n_ =


 81, 31.9%). Free-text responses for motivations to pursue PRS information were generally consistent with results shown in Fig. 1. Although the free-text responses were not formally


analyzed, three main concepts were mentioned by multiple respondents including (i) their personal curiosity about genetic information (e.g., “_Curious about heritability of traits linked to


psychology and personality”_), (ii) personal diagnoses or undiagnosed health conditions which they wanted to learn more about (e.g., “_Currently dealing with medical problems that my doctors


cannot figure out… hoping my DNA would explain_”), and iii) adoptions in the family that drove their investigations (e.g., _“I’m adopted, I have no idea what my family history is… this is a


chance to have some idea”_). UNDERSTANDING AND INTERPRETATION OF RESULTS Regarding general _understanding_ of the meaning of PRSs, 35.5% (_n_ = 93) of participants incorrectly classified


one or more statements (see Table 2). When asked to _interpret_ an example Impute.me result, 61.6% (_n_ = 149) of participants incorrectly identified one or more statements. Overall, only


25.6% (_n_ = 67) of participants answered all understanding and interpretation questions correctly. REACTIONS Given that negative reactions to genetic information are of clinical interest,


we explored the Negative Emotions Subscale further. Average scores for each of the four FACToR subscales are shown in Supplementary Table 1, and overall spread of scores is shown in


Supplementary Fig. 3. We assessed the proportion of respondents who reported that they experienced a negative reaction (upset, anxious and/or sad) either a little, somewhat, a good deal or a


great deal after receiving their personal PRSs. Overall, 60.8% of participants (_n_ = 146) reported that they experienced a negative reaction. Of those participants, 14.4% (_n_ = 21; 8.8%


of total participants) reported that they experienced one or more of those negative reactions either a good or great deal. The mean IES-R score was 5.38 ± 10.0, and most participants (94.7%,


_n_ = 213) had a total score of 23 or below. Of the remaining 5.3% (_n_ = 12), 33% (_n_ = 4) scored less than 33 (indicating clinically significant distress), 17% (_n_ = 2) scored between


33 and 38 (indicating a probable diagnosis of PTSD), and 50% (_n_ = 6) scored above 38 (indicating significant PTSD). Every participant who scored above the IES-R cut-off of 23 also reported


some degree of negative reaction on the FACToR. Overall spread of scores is shown in Supplementary Fig. 3. VARIABLES ASSOCIATED WITH NEGATIVE REACTIONS DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES There was 99%


concordance between sex and gender of participants, and the statistical analyses of these variables resulted in the same findings. No association (all _p_ values > 0.002) was found


between any demographic characteristic and the psychological impact of receiving PRSs (Table 1). Regarding numeracy, the mean FACToR Negative Emotions Subscale scores of participants who did


not get all three numeracy questions correct (mean = 2.5, SD = 3.3) was significantly higher (_t_(43.8) = −3.2, _p_ = 0.002) than those who did (mean = 1.3, SD = 1.6). Reaction scores for


each of the most commonly inquired-about conditions offered on Impute.me are shown in Supplementary Table 2. UNDERSTANDING AND INTERPRETATION OF PRSS We identified significant relationships


between understanding of and reactions to the PRS test as measured by FACToR and IES-R (Fig. 2). Specifically, the mean FACToR Negative Emotions Subscale score for participants who did not


get all three understanding questions correct (2.7 ± 3.2) was significantly higher ((_t_(29.9) = 3.3, _p_ = 0.001) than those who did get all three correct (1.4 ± 1.9). Similarly, the mean


IES-R score for participants who did not get all three understanding questions correct (10.2 ± 14.8) was significantly higher ((_t_(59.6) = 4.1, _p_ < 0.001)) than those who did get all


three correct (2.9 ± 5.3). However, there was no relationship between the number of questions about interpretation of an example PRS result that was correctly answered and either FACToR


Negative Emotions Subscale scores or IES-R scores (_p_ = 0.21, and _p_ = 0.15, respectively). MOTIVATIONS FOR SEEKING PRSS The average FACToR Negative Emotions Subscale and IES-R scores for


the 10 different motivations for seeking PRS information are shown in Fig. 3. The motivations were not mutually exclusive (people could select as many as they liked). We found that


individuals who _did not_ choose curiosity as a main reason for seeking out their PRS scores had statistically significantly higher IES-R scores (mean = 10.3, SD = 16.6) and FACToR Negative


Emotions Subscale scores (mean = 2.7, SD = 3.1) compared to those who chose curiosity as a main reason for seeking out their PRS scores (IES-R; mean = 4.1, SD = 7.2, FACToR; mean = 1.6, SD =


 2.2). IES-R; (_t_(33.4) = −3.8, _p_ ≤ 0.001). FACToR; (_t_(3.8) = −2.7, _p_ = 0.009). We found a small correlation between the number of motivations chosen and higher FACToR Negative


Emotions Subscale scores (_R_2 = 0.122), and the number of motivations chosen predicted 35.5% of the FACToR Negative Emotions Subscale score (_B_ = 0.355, _p_ < 0.001). Similarly, we


found that participants who scored above threshold for PTSD on the IES-R scale chose more motivations (mean = 8.4, SD = 1.0) than those who scored below the threshold (mean = 6.7, SD = 1.8)


(_t_(4.2) = −5.3, _p_ ≤ 0.001). DISCUSSION Our study involved individuals who self-initiated the process of seeking out PRSs by uploading their DTC genetic testing data into a third-party


website to access this information for themselves. To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the motivations for seeking PRSs, understanding and interpretation of the results,


and reactions to receiving this information among this type of user. We found that over 60% of participants experienced some degree of negative reaction after receiving their personal PRSs,


and ~5% scored over the threshold for potential PTSD. We found that having poorer understanding of PRS, being motivated by reasons other than general curiosity to seek out this information,


having lower levels of numeracy, and reporting more motivations for seeking out PRS were related to having a negative psychological impact of PRS information. It is important to note that


the cohort of participants in this study is early adopters of PRS who overcame financial and logistical barriers to seek out this information for themselves. This is relevant to bring


attention to because the participants in our study likely do not represent how the average member of the public would respond to this type of information, and our findings may underestimate


the potential harms of PRSs in a broader population. It is well known that numerical risks are both easily misinterpreted (due to common scientific and mathematical limitations) as well as


subjectively perceived (personal experiences that can impact one’s risk perception) [22]. In our study population, lower numeracy was associated with having a negative reaction to PRS


information. There are unique challenges in communicating PRSs, not only are they probabilistic rather than deterministic, but also they are usually presented as risk stratifications or


relative risks compared to the general population [22]. Theoretically, if people misunderstand PRS results, and assume that lifestyle plays no role in disease risk, this could have important


impacts on engagement with risk reduction behaviors. More information is needed regarding how to effectively communicate relative and absolute risks to patients and consumers, and to


determine if improved communication about risk improves experience with complex genetic information. Our findings suggest that many participants had some understanding about the general


concepts of multifactorial/complex conditions and PRSs, and this has been similarly reflected in other recent studies [23,24,25]. However, many participants in this study had incomplete


understanding and interpretation of PRS, as demonstrated by 74.4% of participants incorrectly answering at least one of the questions in this area. Poorer understanding of PRSs was


associated with experiencing a negative reaction from receiving the information. There are a number of solutions that could be employed to increase understanding and interpretation of PRS


results including more pretest education, changes in data visualization or presentation to make the results easier to understand and more meaningful for users, and involvement of a genetic


counselor or other healthcare providers in pretest and/or results disclosure of PRSs. One example of improved data visualization for PRS communication that could be implemented in the future


is the use of dynamic visuals and additional information about normal distribution curves that is present on this website (http://polygenicscores.org/explained/). Prior to considering the


implementation of PRSs as a screening tool, health systems must consider the infrastructure and personnel required for service delivery without causing patient harm. A major motivation for


participants seeking their PRS in this study was curiosity, and while there are no studies with which to compare directly, this is observation is broadly consistent with studies of why


people upload their DTC genetic testing data to other types of third-party genetic interpretation platforms [23, 26]. We found that when participants chose curiosity as a main motivation for


seeking out PRS information, they were less likely to experience a negative reaction from this information. Similar findings exist in the literature, specifically, in a study of people’s


reactions to receiving _non-PRS_ third-party interpretation of their genetic information, recipients whose motivation was curiosity were more satisfied than recipients who pursued


interpretation of genetic information for health-related reasons [27]. We also found that individuals who experienced negative reactions chose more motivations for seeking out their PRSs


compared to those who did not have negative reactions. A possible explanation for this finding is that individuals who had more unmet needs or higher expectations for the types and certainty


of results they could get from PRS were more likely to experience a negative reaction. Due to the nature of our study, we were unable to explore this relationship further and it could be of


interest for future research studies. This study has several limitations. The demographic make-up of our participants was self-selected and homogenous and the findings from our study may


not capture the breadth of experiences of all individuals, particularly since there are limitations for PRS data in non-white populations further research should be done to specifically


investigate these study questions in a more diverse population. We asked about participants’ motivations for seeking PRSs after they had already reviewed their results, which may have led to


imperfect recall in this section of the questionnaire. Our sample also includes mainly participants who received their results within one week of answering the questionnaire and therefore


cannot be used to draw conclusions about long-term effects of receiving PRSs. Additionally, without the demographic information from non-responders; it is difficult to say whether there may


have been sample bias. It is possible that users who felt more strongly about their results may have been more likely to participate in our study, or conversely, that those who were feeling


more distraught about their results may have been less inclined to participate. Although the IES and FACToR scales specifically ask about and were anchored on participants’ experience with


genetic information, it is possible that there were other confounding factors that contributed to individuals’ having a negative reaction to PRS information that are beyond the relationships


explored through the demographic variables. Lastly, as this was a retrospective study and Impute.me is committed to providing confidential results, we were unable to correlate these


responses with the level of risk for individuals. Future research is needed in this area on more a diverse demographic sample, as our study included those who were self-selected and may have


under-represented harms. It is also possible that there is a more complex relationship between the variables that were explored in this study and future research could undertake a more


complex multivariable model to better understand how these variables may or may not predict a negative reaction to PRS information. In addition, future studies should explore how outcomes


are related to actual and perceived genetic risk levels, which will be important to understand as PRSs become more utilized in both clinical and population-level screening. There is also the


opportunity to perform qualitative research in this area to better understand the individual experiences of receiving PRSs. Lastly, because we have shown that people with lesser


understanding about PRSs are more likely to experience negative reactions, future research should compare outcomes after providing more comprehensive pre-analysis information to consumers.


CONCLUSION The need to understand motivations for seeking, understanding and interpretation of, and reactions to PRSs becomes increasingly urgent as we look to a future where these types of


risk estimates are used regularly in clinical care, population screening, and consumer settings. Higher quality pretest education, particularly to address the limitations of PRSs and more


effectively communicate the differences between relative and absolute risks, may mitigate the risk of negative psychological reactions in response to receiving personal PRSs. Although our


study sample originates from a consumer genetics population, our results could inform the use of PRSs in clinical practice, as the data we have gathered here can help inform the development


of effective communication strategies of PRSs, with the ultimate goal of improving care as PRSs become more prevalent in both consumer and clinical settings. REFERENCES * Torkamani A,


Wineinger NE, Topol EJ. The personal and clinical utility of polygenic risk scores. Nat Rev Genet. 2018;19:581–90. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Lewis CM, Vassos E. Prospects for using


risk scores in polygenic medicine. Genome Med. 2017;9:96. Article  Google Scholar  * NorthShore University HealthSystem. Prostate Cancer Genetic Risk Score. 2016.


https://www.helix.com/products/prostate-cancer-genetic-risk-score (accessed 15 Nov 2019). * Multhaup ML, Kita R, Krock B, Eriksson N, Fontanillas P, Aslibekyan S, et al. White Paper 23-19:


The science behind 23andMe’s Type 2 Diabetes report. 2019. https://permalinks.23andme.com/pdf/23_19-Type2Diabetes_March2019.pdf * Janssens ACJW. Proprietary algorithms for polygenic risk:


protecting scientific innovation or hiding the lack of it? Genes. 2019;10:448. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Folkersen L, Pain O, Ingason A, Werge T, Lewis CM, Austin J. Impute.me: an


open-source, non-profit tool for using data from direct-to-consumer genetic testing to calculate and interpret polygenic risk scores. Front Genet. 2020;11:578. Article  CAS  Google Scholar 


* Heshka JT, Palleschi C, Howley H, Wilson B, Wells PS. A systematic review of perceived risks, psychological and behavioral impacts of genetic testing. Genet Med. 2008;10:19–32. Article 


Google Scholar  * Oliveri S, Ferrari F, Manfrinati A, Pravettoni G. A systematic review of the psychological implications of genetic testing: a comparative analysis among cardiovascular,


neurodegenerative and cancer diseases. Front Genet. 2018;9:624. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Roberts JS, Ostergren J. Direct-to-consumer genetic testing and personal genomics services: a


review of recent empirical studies. Curr Genet Med Rep. 2013;1:182–200. Article  Google Scholar  * Covolo L, Rubinelli S, Ceretti E, Gelatti U. Internet-based direct-to-consumer genetic


testing: a systematic review. J Med Internet Res. 2015;17:e279. Article  Google Scholar  * Stewart KFJ, Wesselius A, Schreurs MAC, Schols AMWJ, Zeegers MP. Behavioural changes, sharing


behaviour and psychological responses after receiving direct-to-consumer genetic test results: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Community Genet.2018;9:1–18. Article  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Bancroft EK, Castro E, Bancroft GA, Ardern-Jones A, Moynihan C, Page E, et al. The psychological impact of undergoing genetic-risk profiling in men with a family history of


prostate cancer. Psychooncology. 2015;24:1492–9. Article  Google Scholar  * Young MA, Forrest LE, Rasmussen VM, James P, Mitchell G, Sawyer SD, et al. Making sense of SNPs: women’s


understanding and experiences of receiving a personalized profile of their breast cancer risks. J Genet Couns 2018;27:702–8. Article  Google Scholar  * Putt S, Yanes T, Meiser B, Kaur R,


Fullerton JM, Barlow-Stewart K, et al. Exploration of experiences with and understanding of polygenic risk scores for bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2020;265:342–50. Article  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Yanes T, Kaur R, Meiser B, Scheepers-Joynt M, McInerny S, Barlow-Stewart K, et al. Women’s responses and understanding of polygenic breast cancer risk information. Fam Cancer.


2020;19:297–306. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Bloss CS, Schork NJ, Topol EJ. Effect of direct-to-consumer genomewide profiling to assess disease risk. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:524–34.


Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process


for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42:377–81. Article  Google Scholar  * Schwartz L, Woloshin S, Black W, Welch H. The role of numeracy in


understanding the benefit of screenning mamography. Ann Intern Med. 1997;127:966–72. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Li M, Bennette CS, Amendola LM, Ragan Hart M, Heagerty P, Comstock B, et


al. The Feelings About genomiC Testing Results (FACToR) questionnaire: development and preliminary validation. J Genet Couns 2019;28:477–90. Article  Google Scholar  * Cella D, Hughes C,


Peterman A, Chang CH, Peshkin BN, Schwartz MD, et al. A brief assessment of concerns associated with genetic testing for cancer: the Multidimensional Impact of Cancer Risk Assessment (MICRA)


questionnaire. Health Psychol. 2002;21:564–72. Article  Google Scholar  * Weiss DS, Marmar CR. The impact of event scale—revised. In: Wilson J, Keane TM (eds). _Assessing psychological


trauma and PTSD_. Guilford: New York, 1996, pp 399–411. * Lautenbach DM, Christensen KD, Sparks JA, Green RC. Communicating genetic risk information for common disorders in the era of


genomic medicine. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2013;14:491–513. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Gollust SE, Gordon ES, Zayac C, Griffin G, Christman MF, Pyeritz RE, et al. Motivations and


perceptions of early adopters of personalized genomics: perspectives from research participants. Public Health Genom. 2012;15:22–30. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * McBride CM, Alford SH,


Reid RJ, Larson EB, Baxevanis AD, Brody LC. Characteristics of users of online personalized genomic risk assessments: Implications for physician-patient interactions. Genet Med.


2009;11:582–7. Article  Google Scholar  * Gordon ES, Griffin G, Wawak L, Pang H, Gollust SE, Bernhardt BA. ‘It’s not like judgment day’: public understanding of and reactions to personalized


genomic risk information. J Genet Couns. 2012;21:423–32. Article  Google Scholar  * Su Y, Howard HC, Borry P. Users’ motivations to purchase direct-to-consumer genome-wide testing: an


exploratory study of personal stories. J Community Genet. 2011;2:135–46. Article  Google Scholar  * Wang C, Cahill TJ, Parlato A, Wertz B, Zhong Q, Cunningham TN, et al. Consumer use and


response to online third-party raw DNA interpretation services. Mol Genet Genom Med. 2018;6:35–43. Article  Google Scholar  Download references FUNDING This study was funded by the


University of British Columbia and was conducted to fulfill a degree requirement as part of training. JA was supported by the Canada Research Chairs program and the BC Mental Health and


Substance Use Services. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Medical Genetics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada Larissa Peck & 


Jehannine Austin * London Health Sciences Centre, London, Ontario, Canada Larissa Peck * Faculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia,


Canada Kennedy Borle * Institute of Biological Psychiatry, Mental Health Centre Sankt Hans, Copenhagen, Denmark Lasse Folkersen * Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia,


Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada Jehannine Austin Authors * Larissa Peck View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Kennedy Borle View author


publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Lasse Folkersen View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Jehannine


Austin View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS LP, KB, and JA contributed to the design of the study, the interpretation of the


data, and the drafting, revision and final approval of the manuscript. LF was consulted on the design of the study and contributed to the revision and final approval of the manuscript.


CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Jehannine Austin. ETHICS DECLARATIONS ETHICAL APPROVAL All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the University of


British Columbia. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. The study was approved by the University of British Columbia’s Research Ethics Board


(H19‐00427). COMPETING INTERESTS LP, KB, and JA declare no conflict of interest. Dr. Lasse Folkersen is the founder of Impute.me but does not make a profit from this service. The voluntary


donations received by Impute.me go to a registered company, from where it is used to pay for server costs. The company is a Danish-law IVS company with ID 37918806, financially audited under


Danish tax law. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER’S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. SUPPLEMENTARY


INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION QUESTIONNAIRE RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Peck, L., Borle, K., Folkersen, L. _et al._ Why do


people seek out polygenic risk scores for complex disorders, and how do they understand and react to results?. _Eur J Hum Genet_ 30, 81–87 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41431-021-00929-3


Download citation * Received: 21 February 2021 * Revised: 18 May 2021 * Accepted: 17 June 2021 * Published: 19 July 2021 * Issue Date: January 2022 * DOI:


https://doi.org/10.1038/s41431-021-00929-3 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not


currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative