Differential effects of purified low molecular weight poly(i:c) in the maternal immune activation model depend on the laboratory environment

Differential effects of purified low molecular weight poly(i:c) in the maternal immune activation model depend on the laboratory environment


Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT The Poly (I:C) (polyriboinosinic-polyribocytidilic acid) paradigm of maternal immune activation (MIA) is most widely used as experimental model for the evaluation of the effects of


gestational infection on the brain and behavior of the progeny. We have previously reported significant batch-to-batch variability in the effects of Poly (I:C), purchased from the same


supplier (Sigma–Aldrich), on maternal and fetal immune responses and found these differences to be dependent on the relative amount of synthetic double-stranded RNA fragments in the high


versus low molecular weight (LMW) range contained in the compound. We here resorted to Poly (I:C) purified for LMW dsRNA fragments to establish a MIA paradigm with increased reproducibility


and enhanced standardization in an effort to refine the MIA paradigm and characterize its effect on offspring behavior. We found that the parallel application of LMW Poly (I:C) in two


different MIA-experienced laboratories (Vienna and Zurich) yielded differential outcomes in terms of maternal immune responses and behavioral phenotypes in the offspring generation. In both


experimental sites, administration of LMW Poly (I:C) induced a significant sickness response and cytokine induction in the pregnant dam and fetal brains, while the expected deficit in


sociability as one main behavioral outcome parameter in the MIA progeny, was only present in the Zurich, but not the Vienna cohort. We conclude that although using Poly (I:C) purified for a


defined molecular weight range reduces batch-to-batch variability, it does not make the MIA model more reliable and robust. The differential response in behavioral phenotypes of the MIA


offspring between the two laboratories illustrates the highly complex interaction between prenatal and postnatal milieus - including the laboratory environment - that determine offspring


phenotypic outcomes after MIA. Consequently, establishing a new MIA protocol or implementing the MIA model firstly under new or changed environmental conditions must include the assessment


of offspring behavior to ensure solid and reproducible experimental outcomes. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS PRENATAL IMMUNE ACTIVATION IN MICE INDUCES LONG-TERM ALTERATIONS IN BRAIN


MITOCHONDRIAL FUNCTION Article Open access 16 July 2024 BEHAVIORAL, NEUROANATOMICAL, AND MOLECULAR CORRELATES OF RESILIENCE AND SUSCEPTIBILITY TO MATERNAL IMMUNE ACTIVATION Article Open


access 23 November 2020 MATERNAL IMMUNE ACTIVATION IN MICE DISRUPTS PROTEOSTASIS IN THE FETAL BRAIN Article 23 December 2020 INTRODUCTION In the past decades, animal models of maternal


immune activation (MIA) have allowed considerable progress in our understanding of the involvement of gestational infection in the modulation of fetal brain development and in shaping


neurobehavioral outcomes in the progeny [1,2,3]. Epidemiological findings have repeatedly implicated maternal infection during pregnancy in an increased risk for offspring neurodevelopmental


disorders, including schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorders, and depressive disorders [4,5,6]. The establishment of reliable rodent models enabled determining the underlying molecular


mechanisms and cellular pathways and to derive causality between immune stimulation of the maternal system and deviation of offspring neurodevelopment and its behavioral repercussions


[7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. One of the most widely used substances to experimentally induce activation of the maternal immune system and to study its effect independently of the immunogenic agent


itself is Poly (I:C) (polyriboinosinic-polyribocytidilic acid). Poly (I:C) is a dsRNA analog whose administration causes a cytokine-associated viral-like acute phase response [1]. The Poly


(I:C) mouse model has been most successfully used in a large number of research studies to determine the pathophysiological principles of MIA and comprehensively delineate its impact on


brain function and behavior of the first, second, and even third-generation descendants [7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. While the Poly (I:C) model has proven to be a very useful and effective tool for


MIA research, it has become clear that the effect of Poly (I:C) in the MIA model may differ significantly between experiments. Recently we have experimentally investigated the observation of


substantial variability in results obtained when using different batches of Poly(I:C) supplied by the same vendor [14]. In a comparative study across laboratories, we found that the


relative composition of dsRNA fragment sizes of low molecular weight (LMW, 100–200 nucleotides) and high molecular weight (1000–6000 nucleotides) determined Poly (I:C) immunogenicity and its


impact on the stability of pregnancy, maternal and fetal immune responses. We concluded that using a Poly (I:C) product with dsRNA fragments purified for their molecular weight may reduce


the variability of research outcomes between different MIA experiments. However, although the standardization of the molecular weight range of the Poly (I:C) product in MIA studies has been


recommended to reduce variability [15], the impact of a Poly (I:C) preparation with defined molecular weight RNA fragments on offspring behavior in the MIA paradigm has remained unexplored.


While previous studies [15] reported more pronounced cytokine response and sickness behavior with HMW (high molecular weight) Poly (I:C) in rats, we decided to investigate LMW Poly (I:C)


only as a refinement measure, since recent studies including mice [14] showed that abortion rates are considerably lower using LMW Poly (I:C) compared to HMW Poly (I:C). Here, we set out to


establish a MIA protocol using a LMW Poly (I:C) product in parallel in two laboratories with ample experience in behavioral phenotyping this model. We tested two different Poly (I:C)


dosages, validated maternal sickness, cytokine response in the maternal and fetal systems, and characterized the behavioral consequences in adult female and male offspring [14, 15].


MATERIALS AND METHODS ANIMALS C57Bl6/N male and female mice (Charles River, Sulzfeld, Germany) 10–12 weeks were used for experimental breeding (see Table 1 for detailed information about


animal numbers). Upon arrival from the vendor, all mice were housed in groups of 2–5 in individually ventilated cages in a specific-pathogen-free (SPF) holding room, which was temperature


and humidity-controlled (21 ± 3°, 50 ± 10%) with a 12 h: 12 h light-dark cycle (Lights on 08:00 AM–08:00 PM (Vienna) and 09:00 PM–09.00 AM (Zurich)). All animals had _ad libitum_ access to


food (Ssniff Spezialdiäten, Germany (Vienna) and Kliba AG, Switzerland (Zurich)) and water throughout the entire study. All animal experiments were conducted in agreement with the EU


Directive 2010/63/EU for animal experiments and approved by the Austrian (License number: BMBWF- 2021-0.150.413) and Swiss authorities (License number: ZH124/2020). Reporting of the animal


experiments is in accordance with the ARRIVE guidelines. All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and their burden. TIMED MATING AND MATERNAL IMMUNE ACTIVATION


Experimental breedings began after two weeks of habituation to the respective mouse vivarium using a previously described timed mating procedure (Khan et al., 2014, Mueller et al., 2018,


respectively). In Vienna, animals were mated for one single night only, and pregnancies were confirmed post-mating days 7–10 by the evaluation of weight gain of the dams. In Zurich, dams


were mated for several consecutive nights, and a plug check was performed each morning. Once the plug check was positive, the dam was moved to a new cage, and the preceding night was


considered the night of conception. The day of the single housing (Vienna) or observation of a vaginal plug (Zurich) was referred to as gestational day (GD) 0.5. Pregnant dams were randomly


assigned to either Poly (I:C) treatment or treatment with endotoxin-free 0.9% NaCl (B. Braun, Switzerland) vehicle solution. For all procedures, LMW Poly (I:C) (cat.#: tlrl-picw) was


obtained from Invivogen (USA). The same lot of Poly (I:C) (Lot #PIW-41-05) was used throughout the study for both laboratories. Poly (I:C) was administered through a single intraperitoneal


(i.p.) injection on GD 12.5 at 10 mg/kg or 20 mg/kg using an injection volume of 10 ml/kg. Immediately after Poly (I:C) or vehicle administration, dams were placed back into their home cages


and left undisturbed until the assessment of the sickness behavior and/or the collection of maternal and fetal tissues. Offspring of Poly (I:C)- or vehicle-treated mothers were weaned on


postnatal day (PND) 21 and littermates of the same sex were maintained in groups of 2–5 animals per cage. For the reinfection experiment, Female F1 offspring (LMW Poly (I:C) and vehicle)


were injected with LMW Poly (I:C) or vehicle on GD 12.5 in the F1 generation resulting in four groups (PIC/PIC; PIC/CON; CON/PIC; CON/CON) for the evaluation of the F2 generation.


Standardized detailed information is provided in the Reporting Guidelines Checklist in Supplementary Table 1 for Zurich and Supplementary Table 2 for Vienna. ASSESSMENT OF SICKNESS SCORE To


assess signs of sickness in response to Poly (I:C) all animals were scored two hours after injection using predefined criteria [16], including body position, ptosis (drooping eyelids),


piloerection, coat condition, reaction to change in environment (cage, assessed by manual interaction within the cage) and nest condition. In both laboratories, sickness scores were


determined by one experimenter each, who was blinded to the experimental conditions. For every mouse and criterion, a value from 0 to 3 was assigned, with 0 indicating no signs of sickness


and 3 indicating the highest degree. COLLECTION OF MATERNAL PLASMA AND FETAL TISSUE 3 h post-injection, pregnant mice were decapitated, and trunk blood was collected in EDTA-coated tubes.


Blood was kept on ice for less than. 20 min before centrifugation (10.000 rpm, 10 min, 4 °C) to collect plasma, which was stored at −20 °C until further use. Fetal brain tissues were


collected as described before [17, 18]. In brief, the abdominal cavity of the dam was exposed to collect the uterus, which was placed into a petri dish filled with ice-cold PBS. Decidual


tissue and yolk sac were then removed for individual fetuses, and the fetuses were further dissected to obtain fetal brain tissue according to landmarks shown in (Supplementary Fig. 1).


Isolated fetal brain tissue was placed in Eppendorf tubes, snap-frozen by immersion in dry ice and stored at −80 °C. Whenever possible, fetal brain tissue from two fetuses per uterine horn


was collected for each pregnant dam by selecting fetuses in the most caudal uterine position. Fetal brains from each dam were pooled and incubated in ice-cold Roche complete lysis buffer


(Complete™ Lysis-M, Sigma–Aldrich, Switzerland) for 20 min before lysing using a tissue lyser (tissue Lyser II, Quiagen®) for 3 min at a frequency of 20/s. After centrifugation (12,000 rpm


at 4 °C for 20 min) the supernatants were removed and frozen and stored at −80 °C until the cytokine assays were performed. QUANTIFICATION OF CYTOKINES Cytokine levels in maternal plasma and


fetal brains were quantified using a Meso-Scale Discovery (MSD) V-plex electrochemiluminescence assay for mice as previously described (Mueller et al., 2018, 2019; Notter et al., 2018). The


panel included interleukin (IL) IL-1β, IL-6, IL-10, tumor necrosis factor (TNF)-α and interferon (IFN)-γ. V-plex 96-well plates coated with primary antibodies directed against the targeted


cytokines were treated with the corresponding detecting antibodies, which were pre-labeled with SULFO-TAGTM (MSD, USA). The plates were read using the MESO QuickPlex SQ 120MM (MSD) imager


and analyzed using MSD’s Discovery Workbench analyzer and software package. Plasma and fetal brain lysates were diluted 2-fold and were run in duplicates according to the manufacturer’s


instructions. The detection limits were 0.11 pg/ml for IL-1β, 0.61 pg/ml for IL-6, 0.94 pg/ml for IL-10, 0.13 pg/ml for TNF-α and 0.04 pg/ml for IFN-γ. BEHAVIORAL TESTING GENERAL INFORMATION


Behavioral testing was initiated when offspring reached 8–12 weeks of age and included tests for sociability (Social interaction test), innate anxiety behavior and locomotor activity (Open


field test) and Fear conditioning (second Vienna cohort only). These tests are widely used in animal models of MIA [5, 16, 19] and extensively validated [17, 20, 21]. The order of testing


was always kept constant (1. Open field test; 2. Social interaction test; 3. Fear conditioning – where applicable) and a resting interval of at least 3 days was kept between tests. OPEN


FIELD TEST A standard open field test was applied to assess basal locomotor activity and innate anxiety-like behavior. The testing arena (40 × 40 cm) was made of white Plexiglas and


surrounded by walls (35 cm in height). The testing room was maintained at fixed lighting conditions (30 lx) and a digital camera was mounted directly above the arena. Activity was monitored


by a computational tracking system (Activity Monitor, MedAssociates, USA) in Vienna, and Ethovision (Noldus Information Technology, Netherlands) in Zurich, respectively. In each case, the


animals were gently placed in the center of the arena and allowed to freely explore for the testing duration of 10 min. For the purpose of data collection, the arena was conceptually


partitioned into two areas: a center zone measuring 15 × 15 cm2 in the middle of the area and a peripheral zone occupying the remaining area. The dependent measures were the total distance


moved (cm) in the entire arena. SOCIAL INTERACTION TEST Social interaction was determined by analyzing the relative exploration time between an unfamiliar congenic mouse and an inanimate


object using a protocol established before [12, 21]. The test apparatus was made of Plexiglas and consisted of three identical arms (Vienna: 37 cm × 6 cm × 16 cm; Zurich: 50 cm × 9 cm × 10 


cm; length × width × height). The three arms radiated from a centrally located equilateral triangle spaced 120° from each other. Two out of the three arms contained a rectangular stranger


cage. The third arm did not contain a cage and served as the starting zone (see below). All animals were habituated to the test apparatus on the day before social interaction testing for 5 


min. During the test phase on the following day, one stranger cage contained an unfamiliar C57BL6/N mouse of the same sex (10–12 weeks of age), whereas the other stranger cage contained an


inanimate dummy object. This inanimate dummy object consisted of three black Lego bricks in Vienna and a black scrunchy in Zurich. The allocation of the unfamiliar live mouse and inanimate


dummy object to the two stranger cages was counterbalanced across experimental groups. To start a test trial, the test mouse was gently placed in the starting arm and allowed to explore


freely for 5 min. A digital camera was mounted above the test apparatus which captured and transmitted images to the EthoVision tracking system. Behavioral observations were made by an


experimenter who was blinded to the experimental conditions, and social interaction was defined as nose contact within a 5-cm interaction zone. For each animal, the relative time spent with


the unfamiliar mouse was calculated by the formula [(time spent with the mouse) / (time spent with the inanimate object + time spent with the mouse)] × 100. FEAR CONDITIONING In the Vienna


cohort, a standard Pavlovian conditioning protocol was employed as previously reported [11]. Briefly, mice were handled by the experimenter on the two days directly preceding the start of


the FC paradigm. On trial days, mice were allowed to habituate for approximately 45 min in a holding room adjacent to the experimental room. The 12 min conditioning training session


consisted of the delivery of three pairings between an unconditioned stimulus (US: foot shock, 0.3 mA, 1 s) and a conditioned stimulus (CS: white noise, 75 dB, 30 s) with an inter-trial


interval of 180 s. Cued recall was tested 24 h after the training session in a test trial lasting 720 s in total, with the preCS phase being 360 s long while the CS was presented for 180 s,


followed by a postCS phase of 180. The response to the CS was evaluated as the freezing levels during the presentation of the CS (preCS-CS). Freezing behavior (percentage of time spent


freezing, defined as lack of movement for at least 0.5 s, or 15 frames at a sampling rate of 30 frames/s) was automatically recorded using the ‘NIR Video Fear Conditioning Contextual Package


for Mouse’ system and accompanying Video Freeze software (Med Associates, USA). STATISTICS Statistical analysis was performed in the R statistical programming environment [22]. For


comparing treatment effects on cytokine concentration in maternal plasma, two-way type 1 ANOVAs with lab and treatment and the lab-by-treatment interaction were used. Separate tests were run


for cohorts of dams receiving 10 and 20 mg/kg Poly (I:C). Behavioral measures and sickness scores were compared with three-way or two-way type 1 ANOVAs with lab, treatment, and sex (not for


sickness score) and all interaction terms as fixed factors. After finding a significant interaction effect of lab and treatment for social interaction, post-hoc two-way ANOVAs with


treatment and sex and the sex-by-treatment interaction were done for each lab. Additional mixed effect models using the package lme4 [23] including dam as random factor showed that dam


explained only a negligible proportion of the overall variance and its inclusion did not affect outcomes for the main factors. We identified multivariate outliers for the behavioral measures


using the package mvoutlier. Re-calculating ANOVA statistics after the removal of these outliers did not change the outcomes for treatment, lab, and lab-by-treatment interaction,


corroborating the robustness of the results. For investigating treatment effects in a new cohort of dams receiving 20 mg/kg at the lab in Vienna, we used two-way type 1 ANOVAs with


treatment, sex, and sex-by-treatment interaction as fixed factors. Re-calculating ANOVA statistics after the removal of multivariate outliers did, again, not change the outcome for treatment


effects. For analyzing treatment effects in the F2 generation after reinfection, we used three-way type 1 ANOVAs with the treatment of dams in F0 and F1, sex, and all interaction terms as


fixed factors. Re-calculating ANOVA statistics after the removal of multivariate outliers did not change the outcome for treatment effects. For comparing treatment effects on cytokine


concentration in fetal brains, two-way type 1 ANOVAs with lab and treatment and the lab-by-treatment interaction were used. Separate tests were run for cohorts of dams receiving 10 and 20 


mg/kg. Assumptions for ANOVA were checked by visually inspecting QQ-plots and scale location plots using the package lindia [24]. QQ-plots showed that residual distributions for cytokines in


dam plasma and brain tissue were partly heavy-tailed but symmetrical. Residuals for social interaction were slightly left-skewed and light-tailed, though given that the observed residuals


fitted the prediction line well over a wide range, data transformation was not considered necessary. RESULTS MATERNAL BEHAVIORAL AND FETAL CYTOKINE RESPONSES UPON LMW POLY (I:C) CHALLENGE As


a first step in the establishment of the LMW Poly (I:C) MIA model (Fig. 1) we set out to determine the immediate immune response of the pregnant female to the Poly (I:C) challenge at GD


12.5 (Fig. 2A). To this end we monitored sickness behavior of the dam and evaluated maternal plasma levels of a panel of cytokines previously characterized in our and others´ MIA studies (eg


[14, 25,26,27]). The selected cytokines comprised IL-1β, IL-6, IL-10, TNF-α, and IFN-γ, which have not only been found dysregulated in the MIA paradigm, but also form part of the


inflammatory profiles in the neurodevelopmental disorders reflected in the MIA model (eg [28,29,30,31]). Three groups (10 mg/kg or 20 mg/kg Poly (I:C) and vehicle controls) of animals were


included in both labs (Vienna and Zurich). We focused on comparisons of either dosage (10 mg/kg or 20 mg/kg) Poly (I:C) to controls (treatment effect), while also monitoring possible


differences between the Vienna and the Zurich sites (lab effect) and interactions between the above (lab x treatment interactions). For sickness behavior we found a significant treatment


effect for the 10 and 20 mg/kg dosage (F(1,33) = 33.758, p < 0.001; F(1,36) = 47.153, p < 0.001, respectively, Fig. 2B). Cytokine analysis revealed highly significant treatment effects


for all parameters tested (IL-6, TNF-α, IFN-γ, IL-1β, IL-10) for both dosages of Poly (I:C)(10 mg: IL-6) (F(1,33) = 88.359, p < 0.001), TNF-α (F(1,33) = 86.434, p < 0.001), IFN-γ


(F(1,33) = 91.698, p < 0.001), IL-1β (F(1,33) = 57.683, p < 0.001), IL-10 (F(1,33) = 54.602, p < 0.001); 20 mg: IL-6 (F(1,36) = 63.535, p < 0.001), TNF-α (F(1,36) = 51.799, p 


< 0.001), IFN-γ (F(1,36) = 35.905, p < 0.001), IL-1β (F(1,36) = 33,197, p < 0.001), IL-10 (F(1,36) = 93.540, p < 0.001). At 10 mg/kg we additionally observed lab effects for IL-6


(F(1,33) = 5.049, p = 0.031), TNF-α (F(1,33) = 4.448, p = 0.043), IFN-γ (F(1,33) = 5.349, p = 0.027) as well as lab x treatment effects for IL-6 (F(1,33) = 7.230, p = 0.011), TNF-α (F(1,33)


 = 6.936, p = 0.013), IFN-γ (F(1,33) = 8.836, p = 0.005), IL-1β (F(1,33) = 4.930, p = 0.033) and IL-10 (F(1,33) = 6.611, p = 0.015). At 20 mg/kg there were only significant effects for IL-10


(main effect of lab (F(1,36) = 12.342, p = 0.001) and lab: treatment interaction (F(1,36) = 16.032, p < 0.001)) (Fig. 2C–G). These data demonstrated that treatment with either dosage of


LMW Poly (I:C) at GD 12.5 elicited sickness behavior and a strong and robust circulating cytokine response in pregnant dams in both research sites with some lab-specific differences. A


summary of all statistical findings is reported in Supplementary Table 3A, B. We next examined the cytokine response in fetal brains (Fig. 3A). We tested the same cytokine panel as in the


maternal plasma using both 10 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg of Poly (I:C). For IL-6 and TNF-α we found significant main effects of treatment for both dosages (10 mg/ kg: F(1,33) = 12.772, p = 0.001;


F(1,33) = 6.562, p = 0.015; 20 mg/ kg: F(1,36) = 12.888, p < 0.001; F(1,36) = 6.862, p = 0.013) (Fig. 3B, C). Significant lab effects were revealed for IL-10 (10 mg/ kg: F(1,33) = 29.545,


p < 0.001; 20 mg/ kg: F(1,36) = 44.433, p < 0.001) and for IL-6 (20 mg/ kg: F(1,36) = 5.469, p = 0.025). Additionally, we observed a lab x treatment effect for the expression of IL-6


in fetal brains of mothers treated with 20 mg/kg (F(1,36) = 4.733, p < 0.036). Levels of IFN-γ and IL-1β in fetal brains were unaffected by Poly (I:C) treatment of either dosage in either


lab (Fig. 3D–F). This observation suggests a potential impact of the laboratory environment on fetal immune responses upon Poly (I:C) challenge of the mother. BETWEEN-LABORATORY VARIABILITY


IN THE BEHAVIORAL PHENOTYPE OF ADULT MIA OFFSPRING Given the expected immunogenic effect on the pregnant dams and significant response in the fetal brains with both dosages of LMW Poly


(I:C), we went on to evaluate offspring behavioral phenotype using the lower dosage (10 mg/ kg) (Fig. 4A). We used social interaction as a main outcome parameter, as we had previously found


a robust reduction of the preference to interact with an alive mouse over an inanimate object in MIA offspring [7, 10, 12]. Total distance moved in the Open field was used as control


procedure to exclude unspecific effects of MIA on exploratory activity as in earlier studies [7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. However, while a significant effect of Poly (I:C) treatment on social


interaction was revealed (F(1,263) = 15.161, p < 0.001) as represented in a reduced percentage of time interacting with the stranger mouse, we also observed a significant interaction


between laboratory and treatment (F(1,263) = 15.072, p < 0.001). Post-hoc tests showed that the expected social interaction deficit in MIA offspring was only caused by differences in the


Zurich cohort (F(1,115) = 47.038, p < 0.001) but not the Vienna cohort (F(1,148) = 0.105, p = 0.746, Fig. 4B). Additionally, we found significant lab effects as well as lab x sex effects


for distance traveled (F(1,288) = 21.437, p < 0.001; F(1,288) = 18.833, p < 0.001, respectively). No differences between MIA and control animals for total difference traveled in the


Open Field were found (Fig. 4C, D), indicating that alterations in activity could confound the readouts of the social interaction test. As such, these data show that the interaction with the


specific laboratory environment may modulate the effect of Poly (I:C) treatment on offspring behavior, despite detectable cytokine responses to Poly (I:C) administration in the maternal and


the fetal compartments. Since no consequences of Poly (I:C) challenge for offspring behavioral phenotype were revealed in Vienna using 10 mg/kg, we next explored whether administration of


the higher dosage (20 mg/kg) would result in the anticipated deficit in the MIA progeny in the Vienna cohort (Fig. 5A). This was motivated by the consideration that the increase in cytokine


levels in the Vienna cohort, which was partly less pronounced than in Zurich, might not have been sufficient to elicit a behavioral phenotype in the progeny. However, no differences in


social interaction between MIA and control female and male offspring were observed (Fig. 5B), and no effect of Poly (I:C) treatment on distance traveled in the Open field (Figs. 5C, D). We


then explored the possibility that an offspring phenotype in response to maternal Poly (I:C) treatment at 20 mg/kg offspring would become overt in a different behavioral paradigm and


assessed conditioned fear, which was previously demonstrated to be altered in MIA progeny [12], but found no effect in the cued fear response (Fig. 5E). Main effects of sex were found in the


Social interaction and Fear conditioning tests (F(1,102) = 26.103, p < 0.001; F(1,102) = 18.839, p < 0.001, respectively). Hence, the lack of a behavioral phenotype in MIA offspring


in Vienna was not dependent on the Poly (I:C) dosage, but may rather be influenced by other parameters of the prenatal and/or postnatal environment. EXPOSURE OF PREGNANT F1 FEMALE MIA


OFFSPRING TO POLY (I:C) (“REINFECTION”) DOES NOT LEAD TO BEHAVIORAL ALTERATIONS IN THE F2 GENERATION We have previously found the effect of MIA on offspring behavioral phenotypes, including


deficient maternal care behavior, augmented depression-like behavior, reduced sociability, and increased fear memory to extend to subsequent generations [11, 12]. In this context, we have


found both, persistence and modification of behavioral traits in the MIA progeny [11, 12]. We thus hypothesized that an effect of Poly (I:C) might be latent in the F1 generation in the


Vienna cohort but could be unmasked in the F2 generation after a possible exacerbation by F1 female MIA re-exposure to the same immune challenge during their own pregnancy (“reinfection”)


(Fig. 6A). To experimentally address this possibility, we generated F1 MIA offspring after maternal exposure to 10 mg/kg Poly (I:C) alongside respective vehicle controls (F0 generation). F1


females (MIA and controls) were divided into two groups, each exposed to either Poly (I:C) (10 mg/kg) or vehicle at GD 12.5, thereby generating four groups of F2 offspring (Fig. 6B). Adult


female and male offspring born to these mothers (F2 generation) were behaviorally examined (Fig. 6B). Social interaction and performance in the Open Field of F2 offspring were unaffected by


Poly (I:C) treatment of either the P0 or the F1 generation dams (Fig. 6C–E). Collectively these results demonstrate that in the Vienna lab adult MIA F1 and F2 offspring do not express the


expected behavioral abnormalities previously observed in the MIA Poly (I:C) model and that “reinfection” of F1 female offspring does not alter the behavior of their offspring under these


conditions. DISCUSSION Purifying Poly (I:C) for dsRNA fragments of a designated molecular weight range has been suggested to reduce experimental variability and support the reproducibility


of experimental results [14]. Here we sought to establish the Poly (I:C) MIA model using a LMW product with a focus on defining offspring the behavioral phenotype most commonly assessed in


studies in the basic and translational psychiatry research fields. We used the opportunity of setting up a new MIA protocol using LMW Poly (I:C) by Invivogen to monitor experimental outcomes


in two laboratories in parallel, to explore whether reducing batch-to-batch variability in the immunogenicity of the Poly (I:C) compound would also contribute to lessening differences


obtained between experimental sites using the same Poly (I:C) product. The phenotypic observation of the mothers (sickness behavior) and the quantification of proinflammatory cytokines in


response to the LMW Poly (I:C) stimulation showed a robust treatment response for both dosages which was also reflected in immune activation in the fetal brains as a prerequisite for


inducing behavioral alterations in the MIA progeny. Yet, the observed differences in the levels of individual cytokines between labs, either as main effect or dependent on treatment/ dosage,


already indicate that the immune response in the model is significantly influenced by the specific animal husbandry and its condition. The current study does not permit identifying if and


which of those distinctions contribute to the resulting offspring phenotypic variability between the two experiments sites. While several behavioral phenotypes in the adult offspring,


including depression-like behavior, reduced sociability, and alterations in fear learning have been reported in the MIA paradigm, we here decided to focus on social behavior as a main


outcome parameter, considering social disconnection/ impaired sociability as a shared symptom of several mental disorders for which the Poly (I:C) paradigm is used as a model in


translational psychiatry research. Moreover, a reduction in social interaction has been robustly observed in earlier studies and across species [7, 10, 12, 32, 33]. However, while 10 mg/ kg


LMW Poly (I:C) at GD 12.5 led to the expected deficit in social interaction in Zurich, no effect was apparent in the Vienna laboratory, despite comparable maternal sickness levels and


significant cytokine induction in the maternal plasma and fetal brains in both laboratory environments. The lack of a behavioral phenotype in the Vienna lab even after increasing the Poly


(I:C) dosage suggests that rather than only the concentration of the compound and the subsequently produced immune response, other pre- and/or post-natal factors may be determining the


effects of MIA on offspring behavior. Notably, while a variety of factors were harmonized across both research sites, including testing protocols for all behavioral paradigms, some


differences between laboratories persisted. These include e.g. the caging system, which can indeed affect cytokine- and chemokine responses, as well behavioral outcomes in the MIA model


[21], the light schedule in the facility (the Zurich lab uses a reversed light-dark cycle), animal food and consequently the microbiome as well as different approaches for generating timed


pregnancies in the two sites. However, these slight variations reflect the typical differences in experimental conditions between research sites, which can and will never be 100% identical


between independent studies. Our data thus indicates that using maternal sickness behavior or levels of selected individual cytokines as a proxy in the establishment of the Poly (I:C) MIA


protocol may be not sufficient for predicting offspring behavioral phenotypes. Both laboratories have independently observed that the effect of MIA on offspring phenotype manifested not only


in the first, but also the second and even third generation, including both persistence and modifications of behavioral traits across generations [11, 12, 34]. Given the highly significant


increase of maternal cytokines paired with a lack of offspring phenotype with the lower dose in Vienna, we tested the possibility that the behavioral effect was latent in the F1 offspring


but could be unmasked by priming MIA offspring with an additional MIA stimulus and testing F2 generation offspring. However, we observed no effect of MIA on offspring behavior in any of the


experimental groups, although we cannot rule out that other “second hits”, such as stress experience or exposure to psychostimulant drugs may uncover or jointly elicit a behavioral phenotype


under these conditions. While in both laboratory environments, LMW Poly (I:C) elicits a robust immune activation in the maternal and fetal systems, the results concerning offspring behavior


differ substantially. These observations jointly suggest that the immune response itself and the interaction with other factors form an intricate web of interrelationships that determine


long-term outcomes in the MIA model. Focusing on a specific set of cytokines when investigating the immunogenic effects of LMW Poly(I:C), as done here as well as in earlier studies [14],


bears the risk that differences in other cytokines, which could potentially underlie the variations in offspring phenotypes, may not be detected. Indeed, other cytokines and molecular


elements downstream of the signaling cascades activated by Poly (I:C), many of which remain to be discovered, may interact with distinct determinants of the specific laboratory setting [14].


Such factors could include, but are not limited to e.g., the maternal microbiome, physical variables within the housing and testing environment (air pressure and flow, humidity) or


postnatal influences within the offspring social groups or influences of experimenters and staff. These may then also not only directly affect the developing fetus, but also indirectly


modulate other relevant variables in the model, such as maternal care behavior [13], which remains to be tested using LMW Poly (I:C). Against this background, our study clearly demonstrates


that using maternal sickness behavior or levels of selected individual cytokines as a proxy in the establishment of the Poly (I:C) MIA protocol may be not sufficient for predicting offspring


behavioral phenotypes. While using Poly (I:C) compounds with dsRNA fragments purified for their molecular weight may reduce batch-to-batch variability, the environmental conditions specific


and intrinsic to each laboratory importantly determine offspring behavioral outcomes for one of the most commonly used models in translational psychiatry. As such, the successful


implementation of a MIA model in each specific laboratory warrants titrating and defining the optimal settings and needs to extend beyond the assessment of maternal sickness and/or cytokine


responses. In this regard, whenever establishing a MIA model or when introducing changes to protocols, the animal husbandry or the laboratory environment, a pilot experiment that includes


not only the assessment of prenatal cytokine profiles, but also the evaluation of offspring behavioral phenotypes, is highly recommended. Verifying offspring phenotype in a small-scale pilot


approach is also favorable in consideration of the 3 R policy, as it will prevent larger cohorts of animals being used with possibly unsatisfying experimental outcomes. DATA AVAILABILITY


The data that support the findings of this study are available upon reasonable request from the corresponding author. REFERENCES * Meyer U. Prenatal poly(I:C) exposure and other


developmental immune activation models in rodent systems. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75:307–15. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Meyer U, Feldon J, Fatemi SH. In-vivo rodent models for the


experimental investigation of prenatal immune activation effects in neurodevelopmental brain disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:1061–79. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  *


Reisinger S, Khan D, Kong E, Berger A, Pollak A, Pollak DD. The poly(I:C)-induced maternal immune activation model in preclinical neuropsychiatric drug discovery. Pharm Ther.


2015;149:213–26. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Brown AS, Derkits EJ. Prenatal infection and schizophrenia: a review of epidemiologic and translational studies. Am J Psychiatry.


2010;167:261–80. Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Brown AS, Meyer U. Maternal immune activation and neuropsychiatric illness: a translational research perspective. Am J


Psychiatry. 2018;175:1073–83. Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Estes ML, McAllister AK. Maternal immune activation: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Science.


2016;353:772–7. Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Herrero F, Mueller FS, Gruchot J, Kury P, Weber-Stadlbauer U, Meyer U. Susceptibility and resilience to maternal


immune activation are associated with differential expression of endogenous retroviral elements. Brain Behav Immun. 2023;107:201–14. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Khan D, Fernando


P, Cicvaric A, Berger A, Pollak A, Monje FJ, et al. Long-term effects of maternal immune activation on depression-like behavior in the mouse. Transl Psychiatry. 2014;4:e363. Article  CAS 


PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Kreitz S, Zambon A, Ronovsky M, Budinsky L, Helbich TH, Sideromenos S, et al. Maternal immune activation during pregnancy impacts on brain structure


and function in the adult offspring. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;83:56–67. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Mueller FS, Scarborough J, Schalbetter SM, Richetto J, Kim E, Couch A, et al.


Behavioral, neuroanatomical, and molecular correlates of resilience and susceptibility to maternal immune activation. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:396–410. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  *


Ronovsky M, Berger S, Zambon A, Reisinger SN, Horvath O, Pollak A, et al. Maternal immune activation transgenerationally modulates maternal care and offspring depression-like behavior.


Brain Behav Immun. 2017;63:127–36. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Weber-Stadlbauer U, Richetto J, Labouesse MA, Bohacek J, Mansuy IM, Meyer U. Transgenerational transmission and


modification of pathological traits induced by prenatal immune activation. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22:102–12. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Zambon A, Rico LC, Herman M, Gundacker A,


Telalovic A, Hartenberger LM, et al. Gestational immune activation disrupts hypothalamic neurocircuits of maternal care behavior. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;1–15. * Mueller FS, Richetto J, Hayes


LN, Zambon A, Pollak DD, Sawa A, et al. Influence of poly(I:C) variability on thermoregulation, immune responses and pregnancy outcomes in mouse models of maternal immune activation. Brain


Behav Immun. 2019;80:406–18. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Careaga M, Taylor SL, Chang C, Chiang A, Ku KM, Berman RF, et al. Variability in PolyIC induced immune response:


Implications for preclinical maternal immune activation models. J Neuroimmunol. 2018;323:87–93. Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Kentner AC, Bilbo SD, Brown AS, Hsiao


EY, McAllister AK, Meyer U, et al. Maternal immune activation: reporting guidelines to improve the rigor, reproducibility, and transparency of the model. Neuropsychopharmacology.


2019;44:245–58. Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Meyer U, Feldon J, Schedlowski M, Yee BK. Towards an immuno-precipitated neurodevelopmental animal model of schizophrenia. Neurosci


Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:913–47. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Vuillermot S, Luan W, Meyer U, Eyles D. Vitamin D treatment during pregnancy prevents autism-related phenotypes in a


mouse model of maternal immune activation. Mol Autism. 2017;8:9. Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Weber-Stadlbauer U, Meyer U. Challenges and opportunities of a-priori and


a-posteriori variability in maternal immune activation models. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2019;28:119–28. Article  Google Scholar  * Bitanihirwe BK, Peleg-Raibstein D, Mouttet F, Feldon J, Meyer


U. Late prenatal immune activation in mice leads to behavioral and neurochemical abnormalities relevant to the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:2462–78.


Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Mueller FS, Polesel M, Richetto J, Meyer U, Weber-Stadlbauer U. Mouse models of maternal immune activation: mind your caging system!


Brain Behav Immun. 2018;73:643–60. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * R: a language and environment for statistical computing. https://www.R-project.org/ (2021). Accessed February 2024.


* Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48. Article  Google Scholar  * lindia: automated linear regression


diagnostic. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lindia (2017). Accessed February 2024. * Desbonnet L, Konkoth A, Laighneach A, McKernan D, Holleran L, McDonald C, et al. Dual hit mouse model


to examine the long-term effects of maternal immune activation and post-weaning social isolation on schizophrenia endophenotypes. Behav Brain Res. 2022;430:113930. Article  CAS  PubMed 


Google Scholar  * Osman HC, Moreno R, Rose D, Rowland ME, Ciernia AV, Ashwood P. Impact of maternal immune activation and sex on placental and fetal brain cytokine and gene expression


profiles in a preclinical model of neurodevelopmental disorders. J Neuroinflamm. 2024;21:118. Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Purves-Tyson TD, Weber-Stadlbauer U, Richetto J, Rothmond DA,


Labouesse MA, Polesel M, et al. Increased levels of midbrain immune-related transcripts in schizophrenia and in murine offspring after maternal immune activation. Mol Psychiatry.


2021;26:849–63. Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Allswede DM, Buka SL, Yolken RH, Torrey EF, Cannon TD. Elevated maternal cytokine levels at birth and risk for psychosis in adult


offspring. Schizophrenia Res. 2016;172:41–45. Article  Google Scholar  * Allswede DM, Yolken RH, Buka SL, Cannon TD. Cytokine concentrations throughout pregnancy and risk for psychosis in


adult offspring: a longitudinal case-control study. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7:254–61. Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Goines PE, Croen LA, Braunschweig D, Yoshida CK,


Grether J, Hansen R, et al. Increased midgestational IFN-γ, IL-4 and IL-5 in women bearing a child with autism: A case-control study. Mol Autism. 2011;2:13. Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed


Central  Google Scholar  * Hughes HK, R.J.Moreno, Ashwood P. Innate immune dysfunction and neuroinflammation in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Brain Behav Immun. 2023;108:245–54. Article 


CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Careaga M, Murai T, Bauman MD. Maternal immune activation and autism spectrum disorder: from rodents to nonhuman and human primates. Biol Psychiatry.


2017;81:391–401. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Machado CJ, Whitaker AM, Smith SE, Patterson PH, Bauman MD. Maternal immune activation in nonhuman primates alters social attention


in juvenile offspring. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:823–32. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Pollak DD, Weber-Stadlbauer U. Transgenerational consequences of maternal immune activation.


Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2020;97:181–8. Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  Download references FUNDING This study was funded by a bilateral grant of the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) and the


Swiss National Science Foundation (SNF): Grant number I 4954 and 310030E_193899 awarded to D.D.P and U.W.S. AUTHOR INFORMATION Author notes * These authors contributed equally: Katharina E.


Tillmann, Ron Schaer. AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Neurophysiology and Neuropharmacology, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria Katharina E. Tillmann, Karin Mueller &


 Daniela D. Pollak * Institute of Pharmacology and Toxicology, University of Zurich-Vetsuisse, Zurich, Switzerland Ron Schaer, Flavia S. Mueller & Ulrike Weber-Stadlbauer * Animal


Welfare Division, Veterinary Public Health Institute University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland Bernhard Voelkl * Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich and ETH, Winterthurerstrasse


190, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland Ulrike Weber-Stadlbauer Authors * Katharina E. Tillmann View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Ron Schaer View


author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Flavia S. Mueller View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar *


Karin Mueller View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Bernhard Voelkl View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed 


Google Scholar * Ulrike Weber-Stadlbauer View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Daniela D. Pollak View author publications You can also search


for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS DDP and UWS conceived the study and procured funding. KES, RS, FS, KM and BV conducted and analyzed experiments. KES, UWS and DDP wrote


the manuscript with input from all other co-authors. CORRESPONDING AUTHORS Correspondence to Ulrike Weber-Stadlbauer or Daniela D. Pollak. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The


authors declare no competing interests. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER’S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional


affiliations. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE 1 LEGEND SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE 1 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 1 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 2 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 3 SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE 2


SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE 2 LEGEND RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing,


adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons


licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a


credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted


use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT


THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Tillmann, K.E., Schaer, R., Mueller, F.S. _et al._ Differential effects of purified low molecular weight Poly(I:C) in the maternal immune activation model


depend on the laboratory environment. _Transl Psychiatry_ 14, 300 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-024-03014-7 Download citation * Received: 13 November 2023 * Revised: 27 June 2024 *


Accepted: 04 July 2024 * Published: 20 July 2024 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-024-03014-7 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this


content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative